Skip to main content
Original Article

An Examination of Circadian Impacts on Judgments

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000420

Abstract. Many people suffer from insufficient sleep and the adverse effects of sleep deprivation are well documented. Research has shown that people’s judgments can be affected by circadian timing. Across three studies, we examined the impact of time-of-day on people’s judgments about hypothetical legal scenarios, hypothesizing that participants responding at a suboptimal time of day (3–5 a.m.) would give higher guilt ratings and be less sensitive to case information (e.g., evidence strength) than participants responding at a more optimal time of day (2–4 p.m.). Although the time-of-day manipulation influenced participants’ self-reported alertness levels, time-of-day did not affect guilt judgments or sensitivity to case information. This research adds to the literature on how extraneous factors may and may not impact probability assessments.

References

  • Adan, A., & Almirall, H. (1991). Horne & Östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: A reduced scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 241–253. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Åkerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 29–37. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459008994241 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Science, 1, 319–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00226.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cappuccio, F. P., Cooper, D., D’Elia, L., Strazzullo, P., & Miller, M. A. (2011). Sleep duration predicts cardiovascular outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. European Heart Journal, 32, 1484–1492. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehr007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Castillo, M., Dickinson, D. L., & Petrie, R. (2017). Sleepiness, choice consistency, and risk preferences. Theory and Decision, 82, 41–73. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-016-9559-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Centers for Disease Control. (2015). Insufficient sleep is a public health problem. CDC Features. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/features/dssleep/index.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chelminski, I., Petros, T. V., Plaud, J. J., & Ferraro, F. R. (2000). Psychometric properties of the reduced Horne and Östberg questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 469–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00208-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Danziger, S., Levav, J., & Avnaim-Pesso, L. (2011). Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 6889–6892. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1018033108 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dickinson, D. L., Chaudhuri, A., & Greenaway-McGrevy, R. (2019). Trading while sleepy? Circadian mismatch and mispricing in a global experimental asset market. Experimental Economics, 23, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-019-09623-0 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dickinson, D. L., & Drummond, S. P. (2008). The effects of total sleep deprivation on Bayesian updating. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 181–190. Retrieved from http://journal.sjdm.org/jdm71113.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dickinson, D. L., Drummond, S. P., & Dyche, J. (2016). Voluntary sleep choice and its effects on Bayesian decisions. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 14, 501–513. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2015.1028064 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dickinson, D. L., & McElroy, T. (2012). Circadian effects on strategic reasoning. Experimental Economics, 15, 444–459. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9307-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dickinson, D. L., & McElroy, T. (2010). Rationality around the clock: Sleep and time-of-day effects on guessing game responses. Economics Letters, 108, 245–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2010.04.029 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dickinson, D. L., & McElroy, T. (2017). Sleep restriction and circadian effects on social decisions. European Economic Review, 97, 57–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2017.05.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Golder, S. A., & Macy, M. W. (2011). Diurnal and seasonal mood vary with work, sleep, and daylength across diverse cultures. Science, 333, 1878–1881. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1202775 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hafner, M., Stepanek, M., Taylor, J., Troxel, W. M., & van Stolk, C. (2017). Why Sleep Matters-The Economic Costs of Insufficient Sleep: A Cross-Country Comparative Analysis. Rand Health Q, 6, 11. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Harrison, Y., & Horne, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision-making: A review. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 236–249. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.6.3.236 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep, 14, 540–545. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/14.6.540 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaida, K., Takahashi, M., Åkerstedt, T., Nakata, A., Otsuka, Y., Haratani, T., & Fukasawa, K. (2006). Validation of the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale against performance and EEG variables. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 1574–1581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.03.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozinski, A. (1992). What I ate for breakfast and other mysteries of judicial decision making. Loyola of Los Angeles Law Review, 26, 993. Retrieved from https://digitalcommons.lmu.edu/llr/vol26/iss4/5 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Pierro, A. (2008). Night and day, you are the one: On circadian mismatches and the transference effect in social perception. Psychological Science, 19, 296–301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02083.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, P. Y., & Marrington, S. (2005). Morningness–eveningness orientation, optimal time-of-day and attitude change: Evidence for the systematic processing of a persuasive communication. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 367–377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.021 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McElroy, T., & Dickinson, D. L. (2010). Thoughtful days and valenced nights: How much will you think about the problem? Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 516–523. Retrieved from http://journal.sjdm.org/10/10618/jdm10618.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McMenamin, T. M. (2007). A time to work: Recent trends in shift work and flexible schedules. Monthly Labor Review, 130, 3–15. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nagel, R. (1995). Unraveling in guessing games: An experimental study. The American Economic Review, 85, 1313–1326. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1992). Explaining the evidence: Tests of the story model for juror decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 189–206. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.189 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pilcher, J. J., & Huffcutt, A. I. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep, 19, 318–326. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/19.4.318 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schoenborn, C. S., & Adams, P. F. (2010). Health behaviors of adults: United States, 2005–2007. National Center for Health Statistics. Vital Health Stat. Series 10, Data from the National Health Survey, 245, 1–132. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 755–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C. S., Folkard, S., Schmnieder, R. A., Parra, L. F., Spelten, E., Almiral, H., … Tisak, J. (2002). Investigation of morning-evening orientation in six countries using the preferences scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 949–968. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00098-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar