Skip to main content
Original Article

Intense Self-Regulatory Effort Increases Need for Conservation and Reduces Attractiveness of Energy-Requiring Rewards

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000395

Abstract. Exertion of self-control produces distinct motivational consequences: the motivation to conserve energy and the motivation to seek rewards. We propose that heightened conservation inhibits reward-seeking, but only when the pursuit of the reward entails substantial energy expenditure. In two studies, we manipulated self-regulatory effort and then had participants engage in an additional task that was either easy or difficult. In Study 1, we found that self-regulatory effort tended to heighten reward-sensitivity but only when the subsequent task was easy. In Study 2, we measured pupil dilation to assess reward sensitivity while participants viewed images of rewarding stimuli. When the need to conserve was intense, we observed reduced pupil dilation for rewards that were energy-requiring but not for those that were energy-giving.

References

  • Amrhein, V., Greenland, S., & McShane, B. (2019). Scientists rise up against statistical significance. Nature, 567, 305–307. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: Self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 670–676. https://doi.org/10.1086/338209 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). The strength model of self-regulation: Conclusions from the second decade of willpower research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 141–145. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617716946 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009). The unconscious eye opener: Pupil dilation reveals strategic recruitment of resources upon presentation of subliminal reward cues. Psychological Science, 20, 1313–1315. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02443.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83–113. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45, 602–607. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burkley, E. (2008). The role of self-control in resistance to persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 419–431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207310458 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2014). Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control: Has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 823. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00823 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Converse, P. D., & DeShon, R. P. (2009). A tale of two tasks: Reversing the self-regulatory resource depletion effect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 1318–1324. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014604 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dang, J. (2018). An updated meta-analysis of the ego depletion effect. Psychological Research, 82, 645–651. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0862-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 76–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denson, T. F., DeWall, C. N., & Finkel, E. J. (2012). Self-control and aggression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 20–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429451 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faber, R. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). To buy or not to buy? Self-control and self-regulatory failure in purchase behavior. In K. D. VohsR. F. BaumeisterEds., Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 509–524). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fennis, B., Jansen, L., & Vohs, K. (2009). Impaired self-control can promote prosocial and health-fostering behavior. In European Health Psychology Society Conference 2009 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not into temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). On taming horses and strengthening riders: Recent developments in research on interventions to improve self-control in health behaviors. Self and Identity, 10, 336–351. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2010.536417 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 352–366. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311411165 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Self-regulation and sexual restraint: Dispositionally and temporarily poor self-regulatory abilities contribute to failures at restraining sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 173–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307303030 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., … Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giacomantonio, M., Jordan, J., Fennis, B. M., & Panno, A. (2014). When the motivational consequences of ego depletion collide: Conservation dominates over reward-seeking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 217–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.07.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., … Calvillo, D. P. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 546–573. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652873 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612454134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 127–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson, V. E. (2013). Revised standards for statistical evidence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 19313–19317. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1313476110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jia, L., & Hirt, E. R. (2016). Depletion suspends the comparator mechanism in monitoring: The role of chronic self-consciousness in sequential self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 284–300. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000108 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahan, D., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2003). Conformity and dietary disinhibition: A test of the ego‐strength model of self‐regulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 165–171. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10132 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (2017). The unappreciated heterogeneity of effect sizes: Implications for power, precision, planning of research, and replication. Retrieved from http://davidakenny.net/recp.htm First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kotabe, H. P., & Hofmann, W. (2015). On integrating the components of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 618–638. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615593382 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–679. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003196 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lakens, D., & Etz, A. J. (2017). Too true to be bad: When sets of studies with significant and nonsignificant findings are probably true. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 875–881. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617693058 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical report). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lurquin, J. H., Michaelson, L. E., Barker, J. E., Gustavson, D. E., Von Bastian, C. C., Carruth, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2016). No evidence of the ego-depletion effect across task characteristics and individual differences: A pre-registered study. PLoS One, 11, e0147770. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147770 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lurquin, J. H., & Miyake, A. (2017). Challenges to ego-depletion research go beyond the replication crisis: A need for tackling the conceptual crisis. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 568. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00568 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M. (2012). Ego depletion: Theory and evidence. In R. M. RyanEd., The Oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 111–126). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., & Neinhaus, K. (2002). Self-control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 113–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.113 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 524–537. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation? Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 14–20. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 160–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.160 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F. Jr., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331–363. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rondeel, E. W., Van Steenbergen, H., Holland, R. W., & van Knippenberg, A. (2015). A closer look at cognitive control: Differences in resource allocation during updating, inhibition and switching as revealed by pupillometry. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 494. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00494 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salmon, S. J., Adriaanse, M. A., De Vet, E., Fennis, B. M., & De Ridder, D. T. (2014). “When the going gets tough, who keeps going?” Depletion sensitivity moderates the ego-depletion effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 647. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00647 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.241 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmeichel, B. J., Crowell, A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2015). Exercising self-control increases relative left frontal cortical activation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 282–288. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv112 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmeichel, B. J., Harmon-Jones, C., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). Exercising self-control increases approach motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 162–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019797 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 770–782. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014635 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261–1280. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.126 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sullivan, G. M., & Feinn, R. (2012). Using effect size – or why the P value is not enough. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 4, 279–282. https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self‐control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tyler, J. M., & Burns, K. C. (2009). Triggering conservation of the self’s regulatory resources. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 31, 255–266. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973530903058490 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Linden, D. (2011). The urge to stop: The cognitive and biological nature of acute mental fatigue. In P. L. AckermanEd., Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications (pp. 149–164). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). Motivation, personal beliefs, and limited resources all contribute to self-control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 943–947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D., & Faber, R. J. (2007). Spent resources: Self-regulatory resource availability affects impulse buying. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 537–547. https://doi.org/10.1086/510228 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11, 249–254. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00250 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, D. D., Altman, M., Boswell, R. G., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2013). Self-regulatory depletion enhances neural responses to rewards and impairs top-down control. Psychological Science, 24, 2262–2271. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613492985 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1494–1508. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203256921 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar