Skip to main content
Original Article

Abnormality in News Stories

Influences on Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000274

Abstract. What makes people show emotional responses toward victims of social injustice in news stories? What are the behavioral consequences of these emotions? Using an online experiment, this study examined the effects of abnormality perception on how people make cognitive and affective assessments of a negative social event. We found that content abnormality (i.e., an unusual/unexpected event) perceived in news stories leads people to attribute less responsibility to the victim and experience stronger emotional responses (i.e., anger and sadness). This in turn increases the desire for a stronger punishment toward the perpetrator. Syntactic abnormality (i.e., negation) indirectly influenced punitive judgment by increasing the responsibility attributed to the offender. The use of negation as the dominant sentence structure (syntactic abnormality) reduced prosocial intentions, but only when the news story was depicted as an abnormal occurrence (content abnormality). These findings illuminate the interconnections between abnormality perception, emotion, and judgment.

References

  • Anastasio, P. A., & Costa, D. M. (2004). Twice hurt: How newspaper coverage may reduce empathy and engender blame for female victims of crime. Sex Roles, 51(9–10), 535–542. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-004-5463-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arkkelin, D., Oakley, T., & Mynatt, C. R. (1979). Effects of controllable versus uncontrollable factors on responsibility attributions: A single-subject approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 110–115. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bagozzi, R. P., & Moore, D. J. (1994). Public service advertisements: Emotions and empathy guide prosocial behavior. The Journal of Marketing, 58(1), 56–70. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224299405800105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 45–62. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420240104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1015–1026. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coleman, J. C. (1950). Abnormal psychology and modern life. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • David, P. (1996). Role of imagery in recall of deviant news. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 73(4), 804–820. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769909607300404 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, R., Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2006). At the boundaries of automaticity: Negation as reflective operation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 385–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.385 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diakopoulos, N., & Zubiaga, A. (2014). Newsworthiness and network gatekeeping on twitter: The role of social deviance. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Weblogs and Social Media ICWSM 2014 (pp. 587–590). The AAAI Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(1), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.1.55 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). From appraisal to emotion: Differences among unpleasant feelings. Motivation and Emotion, 12(3), 271–302. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00993115 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fleras, A. (2011). The media gaze: Representations of diversities in Canada. Vancouver, Canada: UBC Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gault, B. A., & Sabini, J. (2000). The roles of empathy, anger, and gender in predicting attitudes toward punitive, reparative, and preventative public policies. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 495–520. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402772 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 46, 107–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.2.107 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1989). Empathic emotions and justice in society. Social Justice Research, 3(4), 283–311. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01048080 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136–153. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.136 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. KahnemanP. SlovicA. TverskyEds., Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kiberd, R. (2014). The internet of pain: When suffering goes viral. Vice Motherboard. Retrieved from https://motherboard.vice.com/en_us/article/nzee9d/the-internet-of-pain-when-suffering-goes-viral First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kühne, R., Weber, P., & Sommer, K. (2015). Beyond cognitive framing processes: Anger mediates the effects of responsibility framing on the preference for punitive measures. Journal of Communication, 65(2), 259–279. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12151 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, J. H. (2009). News values, media coverage, and audience attention: An analysis of direct and mediated causal relationships. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 86(1), 175–190. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769900908600111 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 1126–1177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 602–616. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N. (1992). A tale of two curries: Counterfactual thinking and accident-related judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(1), 84–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167292181012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1986). Counterfactual thinking and victim compensation: A test of norm theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(4), 513–519. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167286124014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oh, S. Y., & Kim, S. (2016, June 9–13). The power of “no”: How negative frames influence empathy and pro-social behavior. Paper presented at the 66th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association, Fukuoka, Japan First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1(1), 33–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779108401856 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pritchard, D., & Hughes, K. D. (1997). Patterns of deviance in crime news. Journal of Communication, 47(3), 49–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1997.tb02716.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reisenzein, R., Meyer, W. U., & Schützwohl, A. (1996). Reactions to surprising events: A paradigm for emotion research. Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the International Society for Research on Emotions, 292–296. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133–148. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.133 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 5(3), 161–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939108411034 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenhan, D. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1989). Abnormal psychology. New York, NY: Norton. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shoemaker, P. J. (1996). Hardwired for news: Using biological and cultural evolution to explain the surveillance function. Journal of Communication, 46(3), 32–47. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1996.tb01487.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shoemaker, P. J., Chang, T. K., & Brendlinger, N. (1987). Deviance as a predictor of newsworthiness: Coverage of international events in the US media. Annals of the International Communication Association, 10(1), 348–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.1987.11678651 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slovic, P. (2010). If I look at the mass I will never act: Psychic numbing and genocide. In R. RoeserEd., Emotions and risky technologies (pp. 37–59). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turley, K. J., Sanna, L. J., & Reiter, R. L. (1995). Counterfactual thinking and perceptions of rape. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17(3), 285–303. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1703_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vitaglione, G. D., & Barnett, M. A. (2003). Assessing a new dimension of empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and punishing desires. Motivation and Emotion, 27(4), 301–325. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026231622102 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., Van der Pligt, J., Manstead, A. S., Van Empelen, P., & Reinderman, D. (1998). Emotional reactions to the outcomes of decisions: The role of counterfactual thought in the experience of regret and disappointment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75(2), 117–141. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2784 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar