Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000263

Abstract. This study examined the emphasis framing effects of conflicting messages. Japanese undergraduate students (N = 199) received one message supporting whale-fishery competition with another message opposing it (the conflicting-message condition), arguing against the public demand for whale meat (the competing-message condition), or taking a neutral stance on whaling (the unbalanced-message condition). Although the conflicting and competing messages (vs. the unbalanced messages) had no effect on support for the government’s whaling policy, participants in the conflicting-message condition produced more thoughts about whale-fishery competition, had a more moderate belief about the issue, and perceived the importance of the belief to be lower than participants in the competing- and unbalanced-message conditions. These results suggest that messages organized by conflicting frames have unique effects on the availability, accessibility, and applicability of frame-relevant or -congruent beliefs.

References

  • Aklin, M., & Urpelainen, J. (2013). Debating clean energy: Frames, counter frames, and audiences. Global Environmental Change, 23(5), 1225–1232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.03.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Antilla, L. (2005). Climate of scepticism: US newspaper coverage of the science of climate change. Global Environmental Change, 15(4), 338–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2005.08.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bailey, J. L. (2009). Norway, the United States, and commercial whaling: Political culture and social movement framing. Journal of Environment & Development, 18(1), 79–102. https://doi.org/10.1177/1070496508329358 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blok, A. (2008). Contesting global norms: Politics of identity in Japanese pro-whaling countermobilization. Global Environmental Politics, 8(2), 39–66. https://doi.org/10.1162/glep.2008.8.2.39 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Borah, P. (2011a). Conceptual issues in framing theory: A systematic examination of a decade’s literature. Journal of Communication, 61(2), 246–263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01539.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Borah, P. (2011b). Seeking more information and conversations: Influence of competitive frames and motivated processing. Communication Research, 38(3), 303–325. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650210376190 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, P. R. (2002). Framing, value words, and citizens’ explanations of their issue opinions. Political Communication, 19(3), 303–316. https://doi.org/10.1080/01957470290055510 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, P. R., & Gross, K. (2005). Values, framing, and citizens’ thoughts about policy issues: Effects on content and quantity. Political Psychology, 26(6), 929–948. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00451.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bullock, J. G., Green, D. P., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Yes, but what’s the mechanism? (Don’t expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 550–558. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018933 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacciatore, M. A., Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2016). The end of framing as we know it…and the future of media effects. Mass Communication and Society, 19(1), 7–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2015.1068811 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chang, C. (2015). Motivated processing: How people perceive new covering novel or contradictory health research findings. Science Communication, 37(5), 602–634. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547015597914 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.polisci.10.072805.103054 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review, 101(4), 637–655. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000305540707055 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007c). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 99–118. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00331.x First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2010). Dynamic public opinion: Communication effects over time. American Political Science Review, 104(4), 663–680. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055410000493 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Corbett, J. B., & Durfee, J. L. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science media representations of global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129–151. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547004270234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Corner, A., & Hahn, U. (2009). Evaluating science arguments: Evidence, uncertainty, and argument strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(3), 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016533 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Vreese, C. H., Peter, J., & Semetko, H. A. (2001). Framing politics at the launch of the euro: A cross-national comparative study of frames in the news. Political Communication, 18(2), 107–122. https://doi.org/10.1080/105846001750322934 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dixon, G. N., & Clarke, C. E. (2013). Heightening uncertainty around certain science: Media coverage, false balance, and the autism-vaccine controversy. Science Communication, 35(3), 358–382. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547012458290 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dixon, G. N., McKeever, B. W., Holton, A. E., Clarke, C., & Eosco, G. (2015). The power of a picture: Overcoming scientific misinformation by communicating weight-of-evidence information with visual exemplars. Journal of Communication, 65(4), 639–659. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12159 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Druckman, J. N., Fein, J., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). A source of bias in public opinion stability. American Political Science Review, 106(2), 430–454. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055412000123 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Druckman, J. N., & Nelson, K. R. (2003). Framing and deliberation: How citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 729–745. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-5907.00051 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Towards clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication, 43(4), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T., & Dupree, C. (2014). Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to motivated audiences about science topics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(Suppl. 4), 13593–13597. https://doi.org/10.1890/130220 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gerber, L. R., Morissette, L., Kaschner, K., & Pauly, D. (2009). Should whales be culled to increase fishery yield? Science, 323, 880–881. https://doi.org/10.1129/science.1169981 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Japan Fisheries Agency. (2014, May 13). Whales and whaling. Retrieved from http://www.jfa.maff.go.jp/j/whale/pdf/140513english.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Japan Fisheries Agency. (2017, July 17). Whaling affairs. Retrieved from http://www.jfa.maff.go.jp/e/whale/index.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kaschner, K., & Pauly, D. (2005). Competition between marine mammals and fisheries: Food for thought. In D. J. SalemA. N. RowanEds., The state of the animals III: 2005 (pp. 95–117). Washington, DC: Humane Society Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kobayashi, K. (2018). Effects of conflicting scientific arguments on belief change: Argument evaluation and expert consensus perception as mediators. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 48(4), 177–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12499 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kortenkamp, K. V., & Basten, B. (2015). Environmental science in the media: Effects of opposing viewpoints on risk and uncertainty perceptions. Science Communication, 37(3), 287–313. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547015574016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lecheler, S., & de Vreese, C. H. (2012). News framing and public opinion: A mediation analysis of framing effects on political attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 89(2), 185–204. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699011430064 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lecheler, S., de Vreese, C., & Slothuus, R. (2009). Issue importance as a moderator of framing effects. Communication Research, 36(3), 400–425. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650209333028 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149–188. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2804 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 23–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCright, A. M., Charters, M., Dentzman, K., & Dietz, T. (2016). Examining the effectiveness of climate change frames in the face of a climate change denial counter-frame. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 76–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12171 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morishita, J. (2006). Multiple analysis of the whaling issue: Understanding the dispute by a matrix. Marine Policy, 30, 802–808. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marpol.2006.02.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, T. E., & Oxley, Z. M. (1999). Issue framing effects on belief importance and opinion. Journal of Politics, 61(4), 1040–1067. https://doi.org/10.2307/2647553 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221–246. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024834831093 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 243–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Price, V., & Tewksbury, D. (1997). News values and public opinion: A theoretical account of media priming and framing. In G. A. BarnettF. J. BolsterEds., Progress in communication sciences: Advances in persuasion (Vol. 13, pp. 173–212). Greenwich, CT: Ablex. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Price, V., Tewksbury, D., & Powers, E. (1997). Switching trains of thought: The impact of news frames on readers’ cognitive responses. Communication Research, 24(5), 481–506. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365097024005002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roman, J., Estes, J. A., Morissette, L., Smith, C., Costa, D., McCarthy, J., … Smetacek, V. (2014). Whales as marine ecosystem engineers. Frontiers of Ecology and Environment, 12(7), 377–385. https://doi.org/10.1890/130220 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2012). The state of framing research: A call for new directions. In K. KenskiK. H. JamiesonEds., The Oxford handbook of political communication theories (pp. 1–27). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Scheufele, D. A., & Tewksbury, D. (2007). Framing, agenda setting, and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 9–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00326_5.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slothuus, R. (2008). More than weighting cognitive importance: A dual-process model of issue framing effects. Political Psychology, 29(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00610.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Theriault, S. M. (2004). The structure of political argument and the logic of issue framing. In W. E. SarisP. M. SnidermanEds., Studies in public opinion (pp. 133–165). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., Rosenthal, S., & Maibach, E. (2017). Inoculating the public against misinformation about climate change. Global Challenges, 1(2), 1600008. https://doi.org/10.1002/gch2.201600008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Visser, P. S., & Mirabile, R. R. (2004). Attitudes in the social context: The impact of social network composition on individual-level attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 779–795. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.779 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wise, D., & Brewer, P. R. (2010). Competing frames for a public health issue and their effects on public opinion. Mass Communication and Society, 13(4), 435–457. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205430903296077 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yeo, S. K., Cacciatore, M. A., Brossard, D., Scheufele, D. A., Runge, K., Su, L. Y, … Corley, E. A. (2014). Partisan amplification of risk: American perceptions of nuclear energy risk in the wake of the Fukushima Daiichi disaster. Energy Policy, 67, 727–736. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2013.11.061 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar