Skip to content
BY-NC-ND 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Mouton August 13, 2018

CLICS2: An improved database of cross-linguistic colexifications assembling lexical data with the help of cross-linguistic data formats

  • Johann-Mattis List EMAIL logo , Simon J. Greenhill , Cormac Anderson , Thomas Mayer , Tiago Tresoldi and Robert Forkel
From the journal Linguistic Typology

Abstract

The Database of Cross-Linguistic Colexifications (CLICS), has established a computer-assisted framework for the interactive representation of cross-linguistic colexification patterns. In its current form, it has proven to be a useful tool for various kinds of investigation into cross-linguistic semantic associations, ranging from studies on semantic change, patterns of conceptualization, and linguistic paleontology. But CLICS has also been criticized for obvious shortcomings, ranging from the underlying dataset, which still contains many errors, up to the limits of cross-linguistic colexification studies in general. Building on recent standardization efforts reflected in the Cross-Linguistic Data Formats initiative (CLDF) and novel approaches for fast, efficient, and reliable data aggregation, we have created a new database for cross-linguistic colexifications, which not only supersedes the original CLICS database in terms of coverage but also offers a much more principled procedure for the creation, curation and aggregation of datasets. The paper presents the new database and discusses its major features.

1 Introduction

When linguists succeed in identifying similar patterns across various languages, a number of different types of explanations for these similarities are possible. Common patterns might result from coincidence; from natural reasons with a basis in human cognition, psychology or physiology, or indeed the nature of our environment; or they might derive rather from historical processes, among which it is customary to differentiate inheritance and contact. Traditionally, coincidence is not considered linguistically interesting, while natural reasons are the primary focus of work in linguistic typology, as well as providing the theoretical grounding for various universal frameworks of grammatical architecture, while inheritance and contact fall into the purview of historical linguistics. However, to draw a clear dividing line between these two subfields is not necessarily useful or even tenable, as the research results of each feed back into the other, and unpicking which of the various explanations for a given phenomenon is most convincing is not always an easy task. Nowhere is this as clear as in the domain of lexical typology.

Languages differ in how they label the universe and sometimes these labels clash in interesting and informative ways, such that one word form has multiple meanings. This may result from coincidence, termed homophony, whereby multiple meanings for one word form arise accidentally as two word forms come to sound alike, as in French paix ‘peace’ vs. pet ‘fart’, which are both pronounced as [pε]. In contrast to this are cases of polysemy, in which one word form comes to have multiple related senses, as in Russian дерево dérevo, which can denote both ‘tree’ and ‘wood’.

Cases of polysemy may be cross-linguistically frequent, in which case an explanation can likely be found in natural factors, be they linked to some aspect of human psychology or cognition, or the inherent structure of the natural environment (e.g. ‘rain’ and ‘water’, the above example of ‘tree’ and ‘wood’, or the common colexification of ‘moon’ and ‘month’). On the other hand, where a polysemic pattern is relatively rare cross-linguistically, this is likely to point to a historical explanation in common inheritance or contact. For example, many Austronesian and Papuan languages in eastern New Guinea and northern Australia use the same term for both ‘fire’, ‘firewood’, and ‘tree’. As this pattern is rare world-wide, this hints that there might be some deep connection between these groups across the Torres Strait (Schapper, Roque & Hendery, 2016). Another case is given by Urban (2010), who notes that the word for ‘sun’ can typically be translated as ‘eye of the day’ in many Austroasiatic, Tai-Kadai, and Austronesian languages. In spite of the fact that a diachronic development based on a similar equation is attested in Indo-European (e.g. Old Irish súil ‘eye’, from the PIE root *seh2l-, thus cognate with Latin sōl ‘sun’, see 570 Vaan 2008; Classical Armenian aregakn, a compound of arew ‘sun’ and akn ‘eye’, see Olsen 2002), the relative cross-linguistic rarity of this pattern and its prevalence in Southeast Asia suggests an explanation in terms of historical factors.

Deciding on a natural or historical explanation (i.e. distinguishing between homophony and polysemy) may be relatively straightforward for small groupings of languages for which detailed etymological and historical knowledge is available, but it becomes increasingly difficult on a larger scale, and impossible where detailed historical information is unknown. To circumvent this problem, scholars have increasingly begun to use the agnostic cover term colexification, where two senses in a given language colexify if the language uses the same lexical form for both (François, 2008). Taking a colexification approach enables scholars to approach questions of lexical semantics from the perspective of the data: if a pattern of colexification of certain meanings in one language is replicated across different languages or linguistic areas, that is indicative (if not diagnostic) of polysemy, rather than homophony (List, Terhalle & Urban, 2013). However, if frequency is to be used in this way as a proxy to infer polysemy, reliable large-scale cross-linguistic colexification resources are required. The revised CLICS database outlined in this paper is one such resource.[1]

A key underpinning of all colexification studies, whether explicitly or implicitly, are networks, which play a crucial role in the investigation of cross-linguistic colexification patterns. First, they offer a convenient way to visualize the complexity of recurring semantic associations along with a number of high-quality tools for the interactive exploration of network data (Smoot, Ono, Ruscheinski, Wang & Ideker, 2011; Bastian, Heymann & Jacomy, 2009). Second, thanks to recent advances in the empirical study of networks (Newman, 2010), many aspects of network structures are well understood, and a multitude of methods and statistics are available (Csárdi & Nepusz, 2006; Hagberg, 2009), making it easy for scholars to apply them in their research.

Figure 1: Examples for hypothetical cross-linguistic polysemy networks. A shows an unweigh- ted graph. B shows a hypergraph, C represents a network with weighted edges (edge-width representing relative weight), and D shows a network with weighted edges and weighted nodes (node size representing relative node weight).
Figure 1:

Examples for hypothetical cross-linguistic polysemy networks. A shows an unweigh- ted graph. B shows a hypergraph, C represents a network with weighted edges (edge-width representing relative weight), and D shows a network with weighted edges and weighted nodes (node size representing relative node weight).

The application of colexifications in the form of a network is straightforward. Following Cysouw (2010), lexical comparative concepts (Haspelmath, 2010) are represented as nodes in a network, while edges connect colexified concepts. The problem with this approach is that it does not allow one to represent whether a given word form colexifies more than two concepts. In order to represent this kind of information, more complicated network structures are needed, like hypergraphs, in which one edge can connect more than two nodes,[2] or bipartite networks (Newman, 2010, 122), in which nodes are divided into two types and edges can only be drawn between different types (see Hill & List 2017 for an example of bipartite colexification networks). A further enhancement comes from using different edge weights where the weights reflect the frequency of a given colexification in a given dataset, while node weights represent the overall occurrence of a given concept (List, Terhalle & Urban, 2013). Figure 1 gives examples for different possibilities for representing cross-linguistic colexification data in the form of networks.

The promise of a network-based approach to colexification (see List, Terhalle & Urban 2013; Mayer, List, Terhalle & Urban 2014 for colexification analyses and Rosvall & Bergstrom 2008 for general purpose studies) led to the publication of the Database of Cross-Linguistic Colexifications (CLICS, List, Mayer, Terhalle & Urban 2014), which provided cross-linguistic colexification patterns for 1280 concepts across 220 language varieties. While this version of the CLICS database was a valuable resource it also had a number of serious shortcomings. In particular, it had little data, including only 220 languages spoken primarily in South America and Eurasia, and what data were available were hard to check, curate and extend.

In this paper we describe an updated release of CLICS (henceforth called CLICS2) based around a new framework that attempts to solve these problems, while at the same time scaling up the available data, thus facilitating future research into colexifications. The most important points of improvements we see are:

  1. separating data from display,

  2. making exhaustive and principled use of existing reference catalogs like Concepticon (List, Cysouw & Forkel 2016, for concepts) and Glottolog (Hammarström, Forkel & Haspelmath 2017, for languages) along with recently proposed standardization efforts for cross-linguistic data (Forkel, Greenhill & List, 2017),

  3. curating data and code with help of a transparent Application Programming Interface (API), and

  4. regularly releasing data in release cycles of at least one per year (Haspelmath & Forkel, 2015).

In following these design guidelines, we have developed a new database of cross-linguistic colexifications which supersedes the old CLICS database not only in size, both in terms of the number of language varieties and the number of comparative concepts represented, but also with respect to the ease of data curation and the flexibility of the API.

2 How to compare semantics across languages?

Semantic comparison across languages is notoriously difficult. A naive approach to identifying colexification across languages would be to simply map identical translation glosses in wordlists and dictionaries to each other. However, this can easily lead to errors. For example, Chén (1996) originally intended to directly translate Swadesh’s (1952) list of 200 items for Chinese dialects. However, the item dull (knife) was mistranslated as dāi, bèn ‘dull, stupid’ in the Chinese questionnaire. Chén’s version of the Swadesh 200 item list became quite influential in China and was re-used in a number of studies (Ben Hamed & Wang, 2006; Wang & Wang, 2004).

Another approach would be to harvest data for clearly attested polysemies across the languages of the world. Scholars could collect instances of colexifications which they deem interesting, and by using careful hand-collected data, these scholars could control for word meaning and homophony from the start. Such an approach would have the advantage of being extremely flexible in terms of the concepts and the languages investigated. However, the amount of work required for a project of such a nature makes it unfeasible to assemble and curate a global database of polysemies. One project that has attempted this is DatSemShifts (Bulakh, Ganenkov, Gruntov, Maisak, Rousseau & Zalizniak, 2013), which attempts to provide an exhaustive resource on attested instances of semantic shifts across the languages of the world. In its 2015 form,[3] the DatSemShifts database listed as many as 2424 distinct glosses for comparative concepts. The glosses were, however, only minimally specified, which makes it difficult for users to both find a certain concept and to understand what concept they are dealing with, cross-linguistically.

2.1 Harvesting cross-linguistic data with Concepticon

In order to make semantic association patterns comparable across the world’s languages, it is clear that we must base our analysis on a rigorous collection of comparative concepts. The Concepticon reference catalog project (List, Cysouw & Forkel, 2016; List, Cysouw, Greenhill & Forkel, 2018) is an attempt to provide consistent links across the multitude of lexical questionnaires that linguists have used to elicit words. Concepticon works by defining specific concept sets based on published datasets (whether these are from field work, or from historical or typological studies), and then linking the labels used by researchers to these defined conceptsets. For example, the “dull, blunt” vs. “dull, stupid” error is solved by linking the “dull” label in the list of Chén (1996) to the concept set stupid 1518 while linking that of Swadesh (1952) to the concept set blunt379.[4] If possible, these links are further checked against the original data, that is, the words in the target languages that were elicited in the end, in order to make sure that what is glossed as blunt is indeed reflecting the comparative concept stupid.

The immediate advantage of linking data to the Concepticon is that it enables us to merge data from different sources quickly and safely. We now know which word lists contain lexemes for stupid1518 and which contain lexemes for blunt379. And we can now directly ask which languages contain lexemes that colexify stupid1518 and blunt379, and colexify these lexemes with any of the other 3144 concept sets defined in Concepticon. In order to avoid errors, we have striven for rigor and strictness when linking concepts to Concepticon. Concepticon does not tolerate “fuzzy” matchings and deliberately avoids linking one elicitation gloss in a single dataset to more than one concept set in the Concepticon resource. If no ideal concept set could be found to link a given elicitation gloss in a questionnaire, it was left unlinked rather than linking to a semantically “close” concept.

Concepticon concept sets can further be linked among each other with help of a simplifying ontology that identifies concept sets which are broader or narrower with respect to their denotation range. The concept set arm or hand2121, for example, is useful for languages such as Russian or Irish, where the words рука rukà and làmh respectively refer not only to the part of the arm which other languages denote as hand, but also to the entire upper limb. The concept set arm or hand2121 is thus considered broader than either hand1277 or arm1637. While scholars might object to this procedure, preferring to represent a comparative concept reflecting the semantics of Russian рука rukà or Irish làmh by linking them to both hand and arm, it is important to emphasize that this practice, which may seem counterintuitive from the perspective of a given language, is critical if one wishes to guarantee a rigorous mapping of word elicitation glosses in questionnaires to lexical comparative concepts. If a given questionnaire contains the gloss arm/hand (as we can find across many questionnaires which have been used to assemble a large number of data points) and we linked it to both arm1277 and hand1637, we would lose the essential information that the original questionnaire was asking for the word expressing the concept that covers both concept sets in a single term. Since the ontology allows us to derive the information that arm or hand is semantically broader than arm and hand, we can choose, over the course of our analysis, to link the elicitation gloss arm/hand to both narrower concept sets.

Each Concepticon gloss is also linked to additional metadata e.g. a semantic field, ontological categories (reflecting the more language-specific notion of part of speech), as well as additional metadata derived from norm datasets in psycholinguistics and natural language processing, including age-of-acquisition information for individual languages (Kuperman, Stadthagen-Gonzalez & Brysbaert, 2012), ontologies like WordNet (Princeton University, 2010), or word frequency counts, again for individual languages (Brysbaert & New, 2009).

To illustrate how lexical comparative concepts are organized in the Concepticon, Figure 2 provides a small excerpt of the data which is linked to the concept set fat (organic substance)323. As we can see from the figure, this concept set itself is narrower than the concept set organic fat or oil2551, which shows that many languages do not explicitly distinguish oil from fat. On the other hand, the concept set is broader than fat (for nourishment)2095. The definition at the top of the figure indicates that the comparative concept should only be linked to those elicitation glosses which target the organic substance as opposed to potential non-organic variants. The five exemplary elicitation glosses in the table at the bottom of the figure are only a small excerpt of what can be found in the whole data linked by the Concepticon. According to the current version (Concepticon-1.1.0, List, Cysouw, Greenhill & Forkel 2018), the concept set fat (organic substance) recurs in 107 different questionnaires and surfaces in the form of 34 distinct elicitation glosses. As we can see from the five examples in the figure, elicitation glosses can vary drastically, not only because they may be given in different languages, but also because authors do not always pay much attention to consistency. Thus, Swadesh used two different glosses, fat (organic substance) in his list of 1952, and fat (grease) in his later list from 1955, but when inspecting other articles written by Swadesh, we can see that these were absolutely not the only two variants he used, and we find fat in Swadesh (1950) and grease in Swadesh (1971).

Figure 2: Example for the representation of data in the Concepticon project. Note that the three different separators in the elicitation glosses for fat/grease are given as such in the data, thus reflecting the high degree of inconsistency we find in linguistic practice.
Figure 2:

Example for the representation of data in the Concepticon project. Note that the three different separators in the elicitation glosses for fat/grease are given as such in the data, thus reflecting the high degree of inconsistency we find in linguistic practice.

2.2 Using, expanding, and improving Concepticon

We have created a simple automated mapping algorithm to quickly link new wordlists into Concepticon. After applying this algorithm, all links are manually checked to avoid embarrassing errors. In order to further facilitate the task of concept mapping, we wrote a small web-based standalone application which serves as a straightforward lookup tool, including a fuzzy search, across all 3042 concept sets which are currently defined and which can currently be used in seven languages (English, German, Chinese, French, Spanish, Russian, and Portuguese). This web-application, which can be used offline from common web browsers, is provided along with the supplementary material (SI:A) accompanying this paper (see below for further information on the supplementary material). It can also be accessed at http://calc.digling.org/concepticon, where the most recent version is listed. Figure 3 gives a brief example illustrating how it can be used.

Figure 3: Example for the web-based lookup tool for Concepticon mapping.
Figure 3:

Example for the web-based lookup tool for Concepticon mapping.

Given the complexity of the lexical semantics of natural languages, it is obvious that resources like Concepticon or datasets that link to it will contain uncertainties, less-than-perfect links, and even straightforward errors.[5] Concepticon is designed to be easily correctable and welcomes contributions and additions submitted in form of GitHub issues[6] or email inquiries.[7] Reference catalogs such as Concepticon are community efforts that can only be enhanced if the research community actively takes part in improving them and it is advantageous for our field if scholars help correct existing problems.

3 A new database of cross-linguistic colexifications

The first version of the CLICS database was based on four different sources which were publicly available at the time of publication and offered sufficient coverage in terms of comparative concepts. With more and more large questionnaires being linked to the Concepticon resource, it has become possible to easily harvest further data and add it to create an improved colexification dataset. With CLDF as a basic representation format that can be easily manipulated with help of the CLDF API written in the Python language (see Section 4.3), all that needs to be done is to assemble different datasets, convert them to CLDF by linking language varieties to Glottolog and elicitation glosses to Concepticon, and analyze them with the standard algorithms which were already present at the release of the CLICS database.

Table 1:

Overview of datasets converted to CLDF for the new database of cross-linguistic colexifications. Note that the fact that we list 498 distinct glosses for the Bai dataset but 499 Concepticon concept sets, is due to an ambiguity in the English gloss “old”, which occurs two times in this dataset, one time referring to old (aged)2122, and another time to old (used)2113. This information is available in the Chinese glosses, but not in the English ones.

#DatasetSourceRangeGlossesConcepticonVarietiesGlottocodesFamilies
1allenbaiAllen (2007)Bai (ST)498499931
2bantubvdGreenhill and Gray (2015)Bantu43041510101
3beidasiniticBěijrathnang Dàxué (1964)Sinitic (ST)90570018181
4bowernpnyBowern and Atkinson (2011)Pama-Nyungan3483381701681
5hubercolumbianHuber and Reed (1992)Colombian374343696516
6idsKey and Comrie (2016)World-wide1310130532127660
7kraftchadicKraft (1981)Chadic43442867603
8northeuralexDellert and Jäger (2017)North-Eurasian101694010710721
9robinsonapRobinson and Holton (2012)Alor-Pantar39839313131
10satterthwaitetbSatterthwaite-Phillips (2011)Sino-Tibetan42341818181
11suntbSūn (1991)Sino-Tibetan100490548481
12tlsNurse and Phillipson (1975)Tanzanian1589808120971
13tryonsolomonTryon and Hackmann (1983)Solomon Islands324311111965
14woldHaspelmath and Tadmor (2009)World-wide14601457414124
15zgraggenmadangZ’graggen (1980abcd)Madang38030698981
TOTAL / Overlap24871220102890

Table 1 lists all datasets that were selected for the first version of our improved colexification database (http://clics.clld.org). All datasets are multilingual wordlists in the sense of List (2014, 23f). They are based on a collection of (elicitation) glosses that are translated into different language varieties. In our CLDF representation of the data, we have linked the elicitation glosses to Concepticon Concept sets, and the language varieties to Glottocodes. Since not all elicitation glosses could be successfully linked to Concepticon, the number of links to Concepticon and the number of original elicitation glosses in the respective datasets shown in the table often vary, showing fewer Concepticon links than elicitation glosses in the original data. The number of varieties and Glottolog entries also varies, but for different reasons, since it can happen that two or more varieties are linked to the same Glottocode, either because the varieties can be seen as identical but stemming from different datasets, or because the Glottocodes for the subvarieties of a given language or dialect are not yet available in the Glottolog project.

As can easily be seen, the data crucially improves upon the old database in terms of languages. The overlap in terms of concepts, however, is less promising, since many of the lists we assembled are in the range of 300 to 500 concepts. Since these sum up to 2487 different concept sets in total, while none of the original lists provides that many concepts, it is also clear that our new sample is considerably skewed, with only a few concepts recurring across all datasets. However, the framework we have implemented will enable us to rapidly increase the size of this database and improve the coverage via a series of “rolling releases”. Furthermore, interested users can easily use our framework to analyze more balanced sub-samples of the data (for technical details, see Section 4.5) or even feed their own data into our framework.

3.1 General statistics

Our new database automatically derives colexifications for 1220 language varieties (of which 1029 are distinguished by Glottolog) and a total of 2487 distinct Concepticon concept sets. Based on a strict threshold that only accepts a given colexification if it occurs in at least three different language families (as defined by Glottolog),[8] this results in a total of 2638 different colexification patterns, corresponding to 66140 individual instances of colexifications in the languages in our sample. Partitioning the colexification network with the help of the Infomap algorithm (Rosvall & Bergstrom, 2008) resulted in 248 different communities consisting of more than one concept.[9] Given that some of the concepts in our data are never colexified (probably due to the sparseness of the data) our colexification network consists of 1534 different concepts.

Table 2:

The ten most frequently recurring colexifications encountered in our database.

ID AConcept AID BConcept BFamiliesLanguagesWords
1370MONTH1313MOON56289294
1803WOOD906TREE55211310
1258FINGERNAIL72CLAW50209216
2267SON-IN-LAW (OF MAN)2266SON-IN-LAW (OF WOMAN)49262285
2265DAUGHTER-IN-LAW (OF MAN)2264DAUGHTER-IN-LAW (OF WOMAN)47235262
1608LISTEN1408HEAR47102105
763SKIN629LEATHER46233255
2259FLESH634MEAT46222232
1599WORD1307LANGUAGE459498
1228EARTH (SOIL)626LAND43158181
Figure 4: The current coverage of our colexification database in terms of language varieties. Colors indicate the major genetic subgroupings of the languages, following the Glotto- log classification.
Figure 4:

The current coverage of our colexification database in terms of language varieties. Colors indicate the major genetic subgroupings of the languages, following the Glotto- log classification.

In Table 2, we list the most frequently recurring colexifications in our database, sorted by the attestation per language family along with detailed counts on distinct languages and distinct words that attest the colexification. As can be seen from the table, the results are not particularly surprising. It is well-known that many languages use the same word for moon and month or for wood and tree. That kinship terms are particularly heavily colexified clearly results from underspecification and does not reflect any potential instances of semantic shift. After manually inspecting the data to look for potential errors, we are quite confident that the unbalanced distribution of our data did not lead to any major errors, providing the same look and feel as the original CLICS database, while offering a drastically increased quantity of data.

The increase in data is also reflected when one inspects the geographic distribution of languages covered in our colexification database. As can be seen from the map in Figure 4, our colexification data has drastically reduced the number of empty areas, which were so characteristic of the original CLICS database. However, this does not mean that the data could not be further improved. We can still find many areas on the map, especially in Africa and North America, but also in the Pacific and South Asia, in which coverage is poor to non-existent. We hope to improve the geographical coverage in further releases.

Figure 5: Bird’s eye view of our new colexification data in CLICS2. The graph shows all connected components (113) with some of the communities highlighted.
Figure 5:

Bird’s eye view of our new colexification data in CLICS2. The graph shows all connected components (113) with some of the communities highlighted.

3.2 Exploring colexifications through web-based applications

Our application (hosted at http://clics.clld.org) allows users to explore the data from various additional perspectives, including geographic maps, the inspection of the data of individual languages, or the distribution of concepts for which we find translations in our data. In addition, each data point can be traced back to its original source, allowing the users to rigorously check whether the automatic findings we present can be confirmed through qualitative research. In order to illustrate the new application, a number of examples follow.

3.2.1 Bird’s eye view of the colexification data

Before going into details, a bird’s eye view of the data is given in Figure 5. While we can see that most of the nodes in our graph form a large connected component, we can also see that the community detection algorithm singles out communities, adding structure which would otherwise be difficult to spot.

Figure 6: The largest community in our sample, with the concept say1458 showing most connec- tions to the other concepts.
Figure 6:

The largest community in our sample, with the concept say1458 showing most connec- tions to the other concepts.

3.2.2 Colexifications of say, word, and language

Of the 248 communities containing at least two concepts in the data, the largest community consists of more than 20 nodes, centering around the concept speak1623, which has the largest number of links in this subgraph (see Figure 5 regarding the position of the network in our big graph). The detailed network is shown in Figure 6. This subgraph also contains the link between language1307 and word1599, ranking at position nine in the collection of most frequently recurring links shown in Table 2.[10] Those concepts in the figure which are shown in bold font show external links recurring in at least three different language families, clearly suggesting an explanation in terms of natural factors. The concept language, for example, further links to the concept mouth674 which is placed in a community with beak73 as the central concept. The concept set say1458 further links to 11 different concepts from other communities, notably promise1458 (central concept oath1712), call by name180 (central concept shout175), and do or make2575 (central concept build1840). When hovering over the concept in the application, a pop-up provides this information, and users can directly open the respective community to which a given concept with external edges links.

Concepts which could equally well be assigned to different communities are quite common in cross-linguistic colexification data. While this may result from using an inappropriate algorithm for community detection that partitions the network into too small sets of nodes, it also reflects the general indeterminacy of concepts which can often be assigned to different domains. According to our automatic analysis, for example, say1458 plays a role in four different semantic domains, which could be labeled as neutral speech (the community shown in Figure 6), concrete action (community around do or make), promise (community around oath), and articulated speech (community around call by name). Unfortunately, our data is not tagged for semantic fields or semantic domains. If it were, we could automatically derive those concepts which are in transitional areas and not easy to assign to only one domain. Much more work will have to be done in the future, both on existing resources such as the Concepticon, and on datasets in CLDF format, as well as our colexification database, in order to exhaust its full potential.

3.2.3 Colexifications of wheel and foot

As a further example, let us consider a case of regional colexification that was already mentioned by Mayer, List, Terhalle & Urban (2014) and can also be found in our new colexication database: the colexification of foot1301 and wheel710 in some South-American languages. In contrast to the example of say, word, and language in the preceding section, this colexification does not reflect a global pattern which could be identified when looking into the partitions based on the Infomap community detection analysis, which places wheel and foot into distinct communities. An additional view of the colexification data, introduced by Mayer, List, Terhalle & Urban (2014), however, allows one to find areal patterns, provided they are frequent enough and recur across different language families. This view (called subgraph by Mayer, List, Terhalle & Urban 2014) presents the subgraph derived from the closest neighbors of a given query concept. Neighbors of the starting concept are identified by setting a frequency threshold. In consecutive steps, more nodes (the neighbors of the neighbors) can be added to the subgraph, depending on the size of the network, which should not exceed a certain number of nodes to allow for convenient inspection.

Thus, while the colexification between wheel and foot does not show up in our community analysis, we find it in the subgraph view, as shown in Figure 7. As we can see from the different concrete word forms reflecting the colexification, we are not dealing with a direct borrowing that spread among the languages. Instead, the colexification either reflects an instance of loan transfer (in the terminology of Weinreich 1974) or an indirect metaphorical extension. What may substantiate the latter hypothesis is the fact that the wheel-foot colexification is not restricted to Southern America, but seems to be also reflected in some African languages located on the Eastern coast of Africa (Gilman, 1986; Heine & Fehn, 2017), but our current version of CLICS2 does not contain data on these particular languages. The explanation for this particular colexification can thus be sought in historical factors, as a metaphorical extension linked to the introduction of the wheel as a widespread technology in a colonial context.

Figure 7: Detecting regional colexification patterns with help of the subgraph explorer. The col- ors for the languages in the top-left table indicate their genetic relationship. As can be seen, all languages belong to different language families (with three of them being isolates), although they are geographically close.
Figure 7:

Detecting regional colexification patterns with help of the subgraph explorer. The col- ors for the languages in the top-left table indicate their genetic relationship. As can be seen, all languages belong to different language families (with three of them being isolates), although they are geographically close.

This again has immediate implications for ongoing debates on linguistic paleography. First, the wheel-foot metaphor shows that concrete historical events may be reflected in languages. Second, however, it also shows that we need to be very careful when evaluating this evidence. As we can see from the subgraph in Figure 7, there are plenty of colexifications for circle1467 and wheel in our data as well (our data counts 26 concrete colexifications across 11 different language families). Assuming that societies usually have a way to express the concept ‘circle’, while ‘wheel’ may be missing, our data suggests that the most straightforward strategy to express a new concept ‘wheel’ starts from the word for ‘circle’. Since this can easily happen independently, as we can again see from our data, these findings might be of importance for on-going debates on the origin of terms for ‘wheel’, especially in Indo-European (Hock, 2017; Anthony & Ringe, 2015). Further studies on lexical typology, including studies on independently recurring patterns of semantic shift as well as the frequency of loan transfer, are required before this linguistic data can be reliably used to reconstruct ancestral cultures. Our extended colexification data may serve as a starting point for these investigations.

4 Technical background

This section provides interested readers with technical details regarding our improved database of cross-linguistic colexifications. More information can also be found in the supplementary material submitted with this study.

4.1 Shortcomings of the previous version of CLICS

The original CLICS database by List, Mayer, Terhalle & Urban (2014) was compiled in a mostly automatic manner. The data were assembled from four different sources (Key & Comrie, 2007; Group, 2008; Haspelmath & Tadmor, 2009; Borin, Comrie & Saxena, 2013) and were mostly already linked to the same set of comparative concepts, originally based on Buck (1949). The lexical entries were automatically cleaned, using regular expressions and similar standard techniques for text manipulation, and then compared for colexifications. The resulting network was analyzed with help of the Infomap algorithm for community detection (Rosvall & Bergstrom, 2008) in order to decrease the complexity, single out colexifications that point to instances of homophony, and split the semantic network into a meaningful set of subgraphs, representing areas of high semantic affinity, close to the notion of semantic fields (Anttila, 1990).

Since then CLICS has enjoyed considerable popularity among scholars working in the field of lexical typology. On the one hand, this is reflected in studies that mention the database in a favorable manner (Östling, 2016; Georgakopoulos & Polis, 2018; Šipka, 2015), as an inspiration source for similar or enhanced analyses (Söderqvist, 2017; Brochhagen, 2015; Dellert, 2016; Pericliev, 2015; Gast & Koptjevskaja-Tamm, 2018), or as a potential dataset for additional studies (Youn, Sutton, Smith, Moore, Wilkins, Maddieson, Croft & Bhattacharya, 2016). On the other hand, it is reflected in a couple of studies that make direct use of the data provided in CLICS (Schapper, Roque & Hendery, 2016; Koptjevskaja-Tamm & Liljegren, 2017; Staffanson, 2017; Regier, Carstensen & Kemp, 2016). It seems that the general strategy of using an interactive interface that allows scholars to explore the actual data, offering both a bird’s eye view on colexification patterns while keeping in touch directly with the original data fulfils a certain need for studies on lexical typology, serving as an example for computer-assisted as opposed to purely computer-based frameworks (List, 2016).

As mentioned, the earlier CLICS database suffered from a number of shortcomings. Not only was the coverage in terms of languages rather small, with a sample of only 220 languages heavily biased towards Southern America and North Eurasia (Östling, 2016), but the data was also not easy to expand, as new word lists would have demanded a considerable amount of overlap with the 1280 comparative concepts in CLICS. A factor further complicating the expansion of the existing CLICS database was the difficulty in its curation. Given that the data came from independent sources, and that the small number of developers did not have the linguistic expertise to check all wordlists systematically, it was impossible to correct errors in the data itself. Although such curation would have also gone against the original policy of the database, insofar as it was originally built on the idea of providing a different view of already curated datasets, it constituted a serious problem for further development. An additional problem was the transparency of the algorithms underlying CLICS: while the source code was online and freely available, it was difficult to use in its previous state, as it was largely undocumented and provided an unfortunate mix of code written for data deployment and code written for data analysis.

4.2 Software implementation

While the original CLICS framework was deployed via PHP, we have integrated the original code for data visualization (Mayer, List, Terhalle & Urban, 2014) into a Python-based CLLD application (Forkel & Bank, 2018). CLLD offers not only more granular access to the data, but also provides the look-and-feel of well-known typological databases like the Atlas of Pidgin and Creole Language Structures (Michaelis, Maurer, Haspelmath & Huber, 2013) or the World Atlas of Language Structures (Dryer & Haspelmath, 2013). The new framework allows users to inspect the inferred colexifications online via the CLLD framework, and also allows them to curate their own colexification datasets, to analyze, inspect, and even deploy them, thanks to a standalone application which they can use to convert their own colexification data to an interactive visualization very similar to the look and feel of the original CLICS database.

The new database of cross-linguistic colexifications comes along with a simple interface to compute the colexifications and network statistics. To identify and plot colexifications, we follow the strategy employed by Mayer, List, Terhalle & Urban (2014), but have significantly refactored the code, leading to a drastic increase of speed when searching for colexifications. In contrast to the regular expressions by which the data was automatically cleaned in the original CLICS framework, we have decided to use an even more rigorous approach by stripping off all metalinguistic information that can often be found in linguistic datasets (morpheme boundary markers, brackets for scholars’ comments or to indicate pronunciation alternatives) and representing all lexical entries internally with help of ASCII letters. While this carries the danger of introducing errors, our tests indicate that most of these problems can be singled out by only considering colexifications with a frequency above a certain average. Furthermore, since we show the original values as they appear in the data to the users, scholars wishing to work with the data in concrete form can easily check with the original entry or even go back to the original sources of each colexification that we identify with our automated procedure.

We provide platform-independent versions of the Python code which can be used on a Mac, Windows, or Linux computers running either Python 2 or 3. The package provides useful command line tools which we describe in detail at the projects GitHub page at https://github.com/clics/clics2. The software package providing the colexification API is further hosted with Zenodo (see https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1299093 for the most recent version), as well as the 15 datasets (see https://zenodo.org/communities/clics/).

In addition to the CLLD application that provides the data online, we also provide code that exports colexification data to a standalone application, purely based on JavaScript, which can be used locally (and offline) in a web browser, or shared online using a static web server.[11] We assume that this service may turn out to be useful especially for users who want to run their own datasets through our framework, but don’t have the technical means or expertise to set up a complex CLLD application. The new CLICS2 software package also offers information how the standalone application can be computed.

4.3 Cross-linguistic data formats

In order to increase the comparability of cross-linguistic data and to ease the curation and reuse of existing datasets, we follow the standards and recommendations of the Cross Linguistic Data Framework (CLDF). CLDF is a standard to capture different data types often encountered in cross-linguistic research, such as wordlists, typological features, parallel texts, and dictionaries (Forkel, Greenhill & List, 2017). The major features of the CLDF specification are: (A) a simple text format for data-storage, based on CSV (comma-separated values), extended by recent recommendations by the World Wide Web Consortium, allowing metadata to be incorporated and data to be linked across multiple CSV files (Pollock, Tennison, Kellogg & Herman, 2015; Tennison, Kellogg & Herman, 2015), (B) a flexible software API which allows validation of whether a given dataset conforms to the specifications, (C) an ontology which allows frequently recurring objects and properties in comparative linguistics to be recognized, and (D) the rigorous integration of reference catalogs in order to increase the comparability of data across datasets.

4.4 Cross-linguistic reference catalogs

CLDF strongly encourages the usage of reference catalogs, such as Glottolog or Concepticon, when preparing linguistic data. The advantage of organizing language varieties not only by their common name, but also by adding the identifiers offered by Glottolog are obvious. Since Glottolog harvests various types of information regarding language varieties all over the world, ranging from geographical coordinates via references in the literature up to genealogical classifications, scholars linking the languages in their data to Glottolog identifiers can automatically dispose of this information when carrying out additional studies. Disadvantages may result from incorrect links to Glottolog or from problems that experts may encounter when checking the information provided by Glottolog. If, for example, scholars do not agree with the genealogical classification provided by Glottolog, they may prefer to add their own classification to their dataset. However, even if specific information turns out to be erroneous or not satisfying enough for scholars to help in their application, it is still useful to try to provide a link to Glottolog, as it will make it much easier for other scholars to find their data. Furthermore, Glottolog is curated in public and changes can be proposed and made in a transparent manner in the form of GitHub issues[12] or by contacting the editors via email.

As outlined in Section 2, these advantages also hold for the usage of Concepticon as a reference catalog for comparative concepts. While scholars may still use and embrace their individual questionnaires with their preferred elicitation glosses and concept definitions, linking them to Concepticon guarantees that their data is cross-linguistically comparable and easily accessible to other researchers as well.

4.5 Choosing a representative sample using Average Mutual Coverage

A crucial issue when assembling different datasets in the way this is done in our updated version of the CLICS database is whether the overlap in terms of comparative concepts across datasets is sufficient and representative enough. While the data aggregated from the 15 different datasets should be interesting enough for manual inspection and analysis, some analysis strategies (for example those based on hypothesis testing, as mentioned by Roberts 2018) will require balanced and representative samples.

A seemingly straightforward way to identify a representative sample would be to determine the subset that provides the optimal overlap in terms of languages and concepts. In order to get a better idea of how skewed the data in our updated version of CLICS actually is, we carried out a detailed investigation of the different subsets of the data, employing the concept of average mutual coverage (AMC) in multilingual wordlists, as provided in the most recent version of the LingPy software package (List, Greenhill & Forkel, 2017, Version 2.6).

Here, the AMC of a given wordlist is defined as the average of the number of concepts shared between all pairs of languages in a given wordlist divided by the number of concepts in total. Assuming we have a concept list of 100 concepts and three different languages A, B, and C, which have translations for 90, 70, and 60 concepts each, we can determine the average mutual coverage by first checking the individual overlap among the languages (which is not necessarily equal to the number of the concepts translated in the “smaller” of two varieties), and then divide these numbers by the total amount of concepts. If we assume a mutual coverage of 65 between A and B, of 55 between A and C, and 45 between B and C, we can sum up and average the mutual coverage between all pairs. In this example, this would result in an AMC score of 0.55 (0.65+0.55+0.453).[13]

Therefore, if users want a representative sample, they can make use of our AMC statistics, which are provided along with the source code to compute them in the supplementary material, to extract their sample of choice of the datasets we provide in full (SI:B).

We can use this metric to evaluate how well-balanced a given selection of languages and concepts is. This can be done by dividing the data into different subsets in which concepts and languages are consecutively deleted from the data, using their average size (number of concepts, or number of languages which provide a translation for a concept) as a criterion. We can then compute the AMC for each of these subsets and plot the data in order to see how the AMC scores change when reducing the number of languages and concepts in the data.

We carried out this analysis both for our new collection of datasets in CLDF format and the data underlying the original CLICS database. The results can be seen in Figure 8. As can be seen from these plots, our new data collection is heavily unbalanced, with extremely low mutual coverage scores for samples of a large number of concepts and languages. Comparing our data with the AMC statistics for different subsets of the original CLICS database, however, we can also see that our data supersedes the original CLICS coverage for a subset of about 1200 concepts and about 300 languages.

Figure 8: Average mutual coverage in A CLICS, and B our new colexification database. The legend shows the number of concepts for which the AMC has been calculated.
Figure 8:

Average mutual coverage in A CLICS, and B our new colexification database. The legend shows the number of concepts for which the AMC has been calculated.

5 Concluding remarks

Our new colexification database provides a powerful new tool for investigating colexification patterns on both global and regional scales. Thanks to a substantial increase in the data used to identify the patterns, the inference of colexifications is far more robust than before. The new framework based on the CLDF specification and intensive use of reference catalogs has dramatically increased the transparency and replicability of analyses – and our stated policy towards open data and a regular floating release scheme will extend and grow the database further in the future. Our framework can easily be extended following our collaboration guidelines, or co-opted for analysis of alternative datasets as necessary. We see our framework as a central tool for future work in lexical typology.

Funding statement: This research would not have been possible without the generous support by many institutes and funding agencies. As part of the CLLD project (http://clld.org) and the GlottoBank project (http://glottobank.org), the work was supported by the Max Planck Society, the Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, and the Royal Society of New Zealand (Marsden Fund grant 13-UOA-121, RF, JML, and SJG). JML was funded by the DFG research fellowship grant 261553824 Vertical and lateral aspects of Chinese dialect history (2015-2016). JML and TT were funded by the the ERC Starting Grant 715618 Computer-Assisted Language Comparison (http://calc.digling.org). SJG was supported by the Australian Research Council’s Discovery Projects funding scheme (project number DE 120101954) and the ARC Center of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language grant (CE140100041).

  1. Author Contributions: JML wrote the first draft. SJG, CA, and TT revised the first draft. JML and RF wrote the Python code for the application. RF wrote the code for the CLLD application. TM wrote the visualization routines in the standalone and the CLLD application. TT, SJG, JML, and RF provided datasets in CLDF format. All authors revised the last draft and agree with its final version.

Acknowledgements

We thank three anonymous reviewers for challenging and constructive advice. We are grateful to Martin Haspelmath, who brought up the idea to submit this paper to Linguistic Typology and always supported our data-driven approach to lexical typology. We thank Russell Gray for encouraging us to pursue our data-driven perspective on historical linguistics and language typology. We also thank Stéphane Polis, Thanasis Georgakopoulos, and Anne-Maria Fehn for helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper. Last but not least, we would like to express our gratitude to all scholars who made their data publicly available, and specifically to Peter Bouda, Claire Bowern, Johannes Dellert, Michael Cysouw, Gerhard Jäger, Harald Hammarström, Jelena Prokić, Damian Satterthwaite-Phillips, and Malcom Ross for their help in rendering their data in our formats.

References

Allen, Brian. 2007. Bai Dialect Survey. Dallas: SIL International.Search in Google Scholar

Anthony, David W. & Don Ringe. 2015. The Indo-European homeland from linguistic and Archaeological perspectives. Annual Review of Linguistics 1. 199–219.10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124812Search in Google Scholar

Anttila, Raimo. 1990. Field theory of meaning and semantic change. In Günter Kellerman & Michael D. Morrissey (eds.), Diachrony within synchrony: Language history and cognition, 23–83. Bern: Peter Lang.Search in Google Scholar

van der Auwera, Johan & Andrej Malchukov. 2005. A semantic map for depictive adjectivals. In N. P. Himmelmann & E. Schultze-Berndt (eds.), Secondary predication and adverbial modification. The typology of depictive constructions, 393–423. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199272266.003.0013Search in Google Scholar

Bastian, Mathieu, Sebastien Heymann & Mathieu Jacomy. 2009. Gephi: an open source software for exploring and manipulating networks. In Proceedings of the third international AAAI conference on weblogs and social media, Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence. http://www.aaai.org/ocs/index.php/ICWSM/09/paper/view/154.Search in Google Scholar

Ben Hamed, Mahe & Feng Wang. 2006. Stuck in the forest: trees, networks and Chinese dialects. Diachronica 23. 29–60.Search in Google Scholar

Borin, Lars, Bernard Comrie & Anju Saxena. 2013. The intercontinental dictionary series – a rich and principled database for language comparison. In Lars Borin & Anju Saxena (eds.), Approaches to measuring linguistic differences, 285–302. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110305258.285Search in Google Scholar

Bowern, Claire, Patience Epps, Russell Gray, Jane Hill, Keith Hunley, Patrick McConvell & Jason Zentz. 2011. Does Lateral Transmission Obscure Inheritance in Hunter-Gatherer Languages? PLoS ONE 6 (9).e25195. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0025195.Search in Google Scholar

Brochhagen, Thomas. 2015. Improving coordination on novel meaning through context and semantic structure. In Proceedings of the sixth workshop on cognitive aspects of computational language learning, 74–82.Search in Google Scholar

Brysbaert, Marc & Boris New. 2009. Moving beyond Kucera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41. 977–990.10.3758/BRM.41.4.977Search in Google Scholar

Buck, Carl Darling. 1949. A dictionary of selected synonyms in the principal Indo-European languages. A contribution to the history of ideas. Chicago and Illinois: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Bulakh, M., Dimitrij Ganenkov, Ilya Gruntov, T. Maisak, Maxim Rousseau & A. Zalizniak (eds.). 2013. Database of semantic shifts in the languages of the world. Moscow: RGGU. http://semshifts.iling-ran.ru/.Search in Google Scholar

Běijrathnang Dàxué, (ed.). 1964. Hànyǔ fāngyàn cìhuì [Chinese dialect vocabularies]. Běijrathnang : Wénzì Gǎigé .Search in Google Scholar

Chén, Bǎoyà . 1996. Lùn yǔyán jiēchù yǔ yǔyán liánméng [Language contact and language unions]. Běijrathnang : Yǔwén .Search in Google Scholar

Csárdi, Gábor & Tamás Nepusz. 2006. The igraph software package for complex network research. InterJournal. Complex Systems 1695. http://igraph.org.Search in Google Scholar

Cysouw, Michael. 2007. Building semantic maps: The case of person marking. In Bernhard Wälchli & M. Miestamo (eds.), New challenges in typology, 225–248. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Cysouw, Michael. 2010. Semantic maps as metrics on meaning. Linguistic Discovery 8(1). 70–95.10.1349/PS1.1537-0852.A.346Search in Google Scholar

Dellert, Johannes. 2014. Lifting a large multilingual dictionary to the level of concepts. Talk held at the Workshop on historical and empirical evolutionary Linguistics 15–16 February 2014. Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen.Search in Google Scholar

Dellert, Johannes. 2016. Using causal inference to detect directional tendencies in semantic evolution. In Seán G. Roberts, Christine Cuskley, Luke McCrohon, Llu&’ıs Barceló-Coblijn, Olga Fehér and Tessa Verhoef (eds.), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 11th International Conference (EVOLANGX11), Online at http://evolang.org/neworleans/ papers/139. html.Search in Google Scholar

Dellert, Johannes & Gerhard Jäger. 2017. Northeuralex (version 0.9). Tübingen: Eberhard-Karls University Tübingen.Search in Google Scholar

Dryer, Matthew S. & Martin Haspelmath (eds.). 2013. WALS Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/.Search in Google Scholar

Forkel, Robert & Sebastian Bank. 2018. CLLD: A toolkit for cross-linguistic databases. doi:10.5281/zenodo.1186271. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1186271.Search in Google Scholar

Forkel, Robert, Simon J. Greenhill & Johann-Mattis List. 2017. Cross-Linguistic Data Formats (CLDF). Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History. http://cldf.clld.org.Search in Google Scholar

Forker, Daniela. 2015. Towards a semantic map for intensifying particles: Evidence from Avar. STUF – Language Typology and Universals 68(4). 485–513.10.1515/stuf-2015-0021Search in Google Scholar

François, Alexandre. 2008. Semantic maps and the typology of colexification: intertwining polysemous networks across languages. In Martine Vanhove (ed.), From polysemy to semantic change, 163–215. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.106.09fraSearch in Google Scholar

Gast, Volker & Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm. 2018. The areal factor in lexical typology: Some evidence from lexical databases. In Daniel Van Olmen, Tanja Mortelmans and Frank Brisard (eds.), Aspects of linguistic variation, 43-82. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110607963-003Search in Google Scholar

Georgakopoulos, Thanasis & Stéphane Polis. 2018. The semantic map model: State of the art and future avenues for linguistic research. Language and Linguistics Compass 12 (2).e12270–n/a. doi:10.1111/lnc3.12270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12270. E12270 LNCO-0727.R1.Search in Google Scholar

Georgakopoulos, Thanasis, Daniel A. Werning, Jörg Hartlieb, Tomoki Kitazumi, Lidewij E. van de Peut, Annette Sundermayer & Gaëlle Chantrain. 2016. The meaning of ancient words for ‘earth’: An exercise in visualizing colexification on a semantic map. eTopoi. Journal for Ancient Studies 6. 1–36.Search in Google Scholar

Gilman, Charles. 1986. African areal characteristics: Sprachbund, not substrate? Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 1(1). 33–50.10.1075/jpcl.1.1.04gilSearch in Google Scholar

Greenhill, Simon J & Russell D Gray. 2015. Bantu basic vocabulary database.Search in Google Scholar

Group, Logos (ed.). 2008. Logos Dictionary. Modena: Logos Group. http://www.logos dictionary.org/index.php.Search in Google Scholar

Hagberg, Aric. 2009. NetworkX. High productivity software for complex networks. Distributed by the author. http://networkx.lanl.gov/index.html.Search in Google Scholar

Hammarström, Harald, Robert Forkel & Martin Haspelmath. 2017. Glottolog. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://glottolog.org.Search in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, Martin. 2003. The geometry of grammatical meaning: Semantic maps and cross-linguistic comparison. In Michael Tomasello (ed.), The new psychology of language, 211–242. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, Martin. 2010. Comparative concepts and descriptive categories. Language 86(3). 663–687.10.1353/lan.2010.0021Search in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, Martin & Robert Forkel. 2015. CLLD – Cross-Linguistic Linked Data. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://clld.org.Search in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, Martin & Uri Tadmor (eds.). 2009. World Loanword Database. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.Search in Google Scholar

Heine, Bernd & Anne-Maria Fehn. 2017. An areal view of Africa. In Raymond Hickey (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of areal linguistics Cambridge handbooks in language and linguistics, 424–445. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/9781107279872.016.Search in Google Scholar

Hill, Nathan W. & Johann-Mattis List. 2017. Challenges of annotation and analysis in computer-assisted language comparison: A case study on Burmish languages. Yearbook of the Poznań Linguistic Meeting 3(1). 47–76.10.1515/yplm-2017-0003Search in Google Scholar

Hock, Hans Henrich. 2017. Indo-European linguistics meets Micronesian and Sunda-Sulawesi. Wellington Working Papers in Linguistics 23. 63–67.Search in Google Scholar

Huber, R. Q. & R. B. Reed. 1992. Vocabulario comparativo: palabras selectas de lenguas indígenas de Colombia [Comparative vocabulary. Selected words from the indigeneous languages of Columbia]. Santafé de Bogota: Asociatión Instituto Lingüístico de Verano.Search in Google Scholar

Key, Mary Ritchie & Bernard Comrie (eds.). 2007. IDS – The intercontinental dictionary series. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Search in Google Scholar

Key, Mary Ritchie & Bernard Comrie. 2016. The intercontinental dictionary series. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Search in Google Scholar

Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria. 2012. New directions in lexical typology. Linguistics 50(3). 373–394.10.1515/ling-2012-0013Search in Google Scholar

Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria & Henrik Liljegren. 2017. Semantic patterns from an areal perspective. In Raymond Hickey (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of areal linguistics, 204–236. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781107279872.009Search in Google Scholar

Kraft, Charles H. (ed.). 1981. Chadic wordlists. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.Search in Google Scholar

Kuperman, Victor, Hans Stadthagen-Gonzalez & Marc Brysbaert. 2012. Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods 44(4). 978–990. http://dx. doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4.Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis. 2014. Sequence comparison in historical linguistics. Düsseldorf: Düsseldorf University Press.Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis. 2016. Computer-Assisted Language Comparison: Reconciling computational and classical approaches in historical linguistics. Tech. rep. Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History Jena.Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis, Michael Cysouw & Robert Forkel. 2016. Concepticon. A resource for the linking of concept lists. In Nicoletta Calzolari (Conference Chair), Khalid Choukri, Thierry Declerck, Marko Grobelnik, Bente Maegaard, Joseph Mariani, Asuncion Moreno, Jan Odijk & Stelios Piperidis (eds.), Proceedings of the tenth international conference on language resources and evaluation, 2393–2400. European Language Resources Association (ELRA).Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis, Michael Cysouw, Simon Greenhill & Robert Forkel. 2018. Concepticon. A resource for the linking of concept list. Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History. http://concepticon.clld.org/.Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis, Simon Greenhill & Robert Forkel. 2017. LingPy. A Python library for quantitative tasks in historical linguistics. Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History. doi:https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1065403. http://lingpy.org.Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis, Thomas Mayer, Anselm Terhalle & Matthias Urban (eds.). 2014. CLICS: Database of cross-linguistic colexifications. Marburg: Forschungszentrum Deutscher Sprachatlas. http://clics.lingpy.org.Search in Google Scholar

List, Johann-Mattis, Anselm Terhalle & Matthias Urban. 2013. Using network approaches to enhance the analysis of cross-linguistic polysemies. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Computational Semantics – Short Papers, 347–353. Stroudsburg: Association for Computational Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Mayer, Thomas, Johann-Mattis List, Anselm Terhalle & Matthias Urban. 2014. An interactive visualization of cross-linguistic colexification patterns. In Visualization as added value in the development, use and evaluation of linguistic resources. Workshop organized as part of the International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation, 1–8. LREC.Search in Google Scholar

Michaelis, Susanne Maria, Philippe Maurer, Martin Haspelmath & Magnus Huber. 2013. The Atlas of Pidign and Creole language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Search in Google Scholar

Newman, Mark. E. J. 2010. Networks. An Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Nurse, Derek & Gérard Phillipson. 1975. Tanzania Language Survey. Dar es Salaam: Department of Foreign Languages and Linguistics, University of Dar es Salaam.Search in Google Scholar

Olsen, Brigitte. A. 2002. Thoughts on Indo-European compounds - inspired by a look at Armenian. Transactions of the Philological Society 100(1). 233–257.10.1111/1467-968X.00098Search in Google Scholar

Östling, Robert. 2016. Studying colexification through massively parallel corpora. In Päivi Juvonen & Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm (eds.), The lexical tpypology of semantic shifts, 157–176. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110377675-006Search in Google Scholar

Pericliev, Vladimir. 2015. On colexification among basic vocabulary. Journal of Universal Language 16(2). 63–93.10.22425/jul.2015.16.2.63Search in Google Scholar

Pollock, Rufus, Jeni Tennison, Gregg Kellogg & Ivan Herman. 2015. Metadata vocabulary for tabular data. Techreport World Wide Web Consortium (W3C). https://www.w3.org/TR/ tabular-metadata/.Search in Google Scholar

Princeton University. 2010. WordNet. A lexical database for English. Online Resource. https://wordnet.princeton.edu/.Search in Google Scholar

Regier, Terry, Alexandra Carstensen & Charles Kemp. 2016. Languages support efficient communication about the environment: Words for snow revisited. PLOS ONE 11(4). 1–17. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0151138. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151138.Search in Google Scholar

Roberts, Seán G. 2018. Robust, causal, and incremental approaches to investigating linguistic adaptation. Frontiers in Psychology 9. 166. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00166. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00166.Search in Google Scholar

Robinson, Laura C & Gary Holton. 2012. Internal classification of the Alor-Pantar language family using computational methods applied to the lexicon. Language Dynamics and Change 2(2). 123–149.10.1163/22105832-20120201Search in Google Scholar

Rosvall, Martin & Carl T. Bergstrom. 2008. Maps of random walks on complex networks reveal community structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. U.S.A. 105(4). 1118–1123.10.1073/pnas.0706851105Search in Google Scholar

Satterthwaite-Phillips, D. 2011. Phylogenetic inference of the Tibeto-Burman languages or on the usefuseful of lexicostatistics (and “megalo”-comparison) for the subgrouping of Tibeto-Burman. Stanford: Stanford University Phd thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Schapper, Antoinette, Lila San Roque & Rachel Hendery. 2016. Tree, firewood and fire in the languages of Sahul. In Päivi Juvonen & Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm (eds.), The lexical typology of semantic shifts, 355–422. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110377675-012Search in Google Scholar

Söderqvist, Kajsa. 2017. Colexification and semantic change in colour terms in Sino-Tibetan and Indo-European languages. Lund: University of Lund Bachelor’s thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Smoot, Michael E., Keiichiro Ono, Johannes Ruscheinski, Peng-Liang Wang & Trey Ideker. 2011. Cytoscape 2.8. Bioinformatics 27(3). 431–432.10.1093/bioinformatics/btq675Search in Google Scholar

Sūn, Hóngkāi (ed.). 1991. Zàngmiǎnyǔ yǔyrathnan hé cíhuì [Tibeto-Burman phonology and lexicon]. Běijrathnang: Zhōngguó Shèhuì Kēxué [Chinese Social Sciences Press].Search in Google Scholar

Staffanson, Martina. 2017. Mitt hjärta är bittert. En lexikal typologisk studie om smaktermer. Stockholm: Institut för Lingvistik Term paper.Search in Google Scholar

Swadesh, Morris. 1950. Salish internal relationships. International Journal of American Linguistics 16(4). 157–167.10.1086/464084Search in Google Scholar

Swadesh, Morris. 1952. Lexico-statistic dating of prehistoric ethnic contacts. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 96(4). 452–463.Search in Google Scholar

Swadesh, Morris. 1955. Towards greater accuracy in lexicostatistic dating. International Journal of American Linguistics 21(2). 121–137.10.1086/464321Search in Google Scholar

Swadesh, Morris. 1971. The origin and diversification of language: Edited post mortem by Joel Sherzer. Chicago: Aldine.Search in Google Scholar

Tennison, Jeni, Gregg Kellogg & Ivan Herman. 2015. Model for tabular data and metadata on the web. Techreport World Wide Web Consortium (W3C). https://www.w3.org/TR/tabular- data-model/.Search in Google Scholar

Tryon, Darrel T. & Brian D. Hackman. 1983. Solomon Islands languages. An internal classification (C 72). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Urban, Matthias. 2010. ‘Sun’=‘Eye of the Day’: A linguistic pattern of Southeast Asia and Oceania. Oceanic Linguistics 49(2). 568–579.Search in Google Scholar

Vaan, Michiel. 2008. Indo-European linguistics. LINGUA 118(8). 1228–1232.10.1016/j.lingua.2007.12.002Search in Google Scholar

Šipka, Danko. 2015. Lexical conflict. Theory and practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781316337004Search in Google Scholar

Wang, Feng & William S.-Y. Wang. 2004. Basic words and language evolution. Language and Linguistics 5(3). 643–662.Search in Google Scholar

Weinreich, Uriel. 1974. Languages in contact. With a preface by André Martinet. The Hague and Paris: Mouton 8th edn.Search in Google Scholar

Youn, Hyejin, Logan Sutton, Eric Smith, Cristopher Moore, Jon F. Wilkins, Ian Maddieson, William Croft & Tanmoy Bhattacharya. 2016. On the universal structure of human lexical semantics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America doi:10.1073/pnas.1520752113.Search in Google Scholar

Z’graggen, John A. 1980a. A comparative word list of the Mabuso languages, Madang Province, Papua New Guinea. Canberra, Australia: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Z’graggen, John A. 1980b. A comparative word list of the Northern Adelbert Range languages, Madang Province, Papua New Guinea. Canberra, Australia: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Z’graggen, John A. 1980c. A comparative word list of the Rai Coast languages, Madang province, Papua New Guinea. Canberra, Australia: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Z’graggen, JA. 1980d. A comparative word list of the Southern Adelbert Range languages, Papua New Guinea. Canberra, Australia: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar


Supplementary Material

The online version of this article offers supplementary material (DOI:https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2018-0010).


Received: 2018-03-07
Revised: 2018-05-17
Published Online: 2018-08-13
Published in Print: 2018-08-28

© 2018 List et al., published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 License.

Downloaded on 19.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/lingty-2018-0010/html
Scroll to top button