Hostname: page-component-6b989bf9dc-lb7rp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-14T11:22:11.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Eye-movement benchmarks in Heritage Language reading

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2020

Olga Parshina*
Affiliation:
College of Staten Island and The Graduate Center of The City University of New York
Anna K. Laurinavichyute
Affiliation:
National Research University Higher School of Economics, Russian Federation University of Potsdam, Germany
Irina A. Sekerina
Affiliation:
College of Staten Island and The Graduate Center of The City University of New York National Research University Higher School of Economics, Russian Federation
*
Address for correspondence: Olga Parshina, E-mail: oparshina@gradcenter.cuny.edu

Abstract

This eye-tracking study establishes basic benchmarks of eye movements during reading in heritage language (HL) by Russian-speaking adults and adolescents of high (n = 21) and low proficiency (n = 27). Heritage speakers (HSs) read sentences in Cyrillic, and their eye movements were compared to those of Russian monolingual skilled adult readers, 8-year-old children and L2 learners. Reading patterns of HSs revealed longer mean fixation durations, lower skipping probabilities, and higher regressive saccade rates than in monolingual adults. High-proficient HSs were more similar to monolingual children, while low-proficient HSs performed on par with L2 learners. Low-proficient HSs differed from high-proficient HSs in exhibiting lower skipping probabilities, higher fixation counts, and larger frequency effects. Taken together, our findings are consistent with the weaker links account of bilingual language processing as well as the divergent attainment theory of HL.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barnes, AE and Kim, YS (2016) Low-skilled adult readers look like typically developing child readers: a comparison of reading skills and eye movement behavior. Reading and Writing 29, 18891914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Mächler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benmamoun, E, Montrul, S and Polinsky, M (2013) Heritage languages and their speakers: Opportunities and challenges for linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics 39, 129181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berzak, Y, Katz, B and Levy, R (2018) Assessing language proficiency from eye movements in reading. In Walker, MA, Ji, H & Stent, A (eds), Proceedings of the 2018 Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human language technologies. New Orleans, LA: Association for Computational Linguistics, pp. 19861996.Google Scholar
Blythe, HI and Joseph, H. S. S. L. (2011) Children's eye movements during reading. In Liversedge, SP, Gilchrist, ID & Everling, S (eds), The Oxford handbook of eye movements. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 643662.Google Scholar
Blythe, HI, Häikiö, T, Bertam, R, Liversedge, SP and Hyönä, J (2011) Reading disappearing text: Why do children refixate words?. Vision Research 51, 8492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boston, MF, Hale, J, Kliegl, R, Patil, U and Vasishth, S (2008) Parsing costs as predictors of reading difficulty: An evaluation using the Potsdam Sentence Corpus. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2, 112.Google Scholar
Carreira, M and Kagan, O (2011) The results of the National Heritage Language Survey: Implications for teaching, curriculum design, and professional development. Foreign Language Annals 44, 4064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifton, C Jr., Staub, A and Rayner, K (2007) Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In van Gompel, RPG, Fischer, MH, Murray, WS & Hill, RL (eds), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 341369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cop, U, Drieghe, D and Duyck, W (2015) Eye movement patterns in natural reading: A comparison of monolingual and bilingual reading of a novel. PLoS ONE 10, e0134008. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0134008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cop, U, Dirix, N, Drieghe, D and Duyck, W (2017) Presenting GECO: An eyetracking corpus of monolingual and bilingual sentence reading. Behavior Research Methods 49, 602615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drieghe, D (2011) Parafoveal-on-foveal effects in eye movements during reading. In Liversedge, SP, Gilchrist, ID and Everling, S (eds), Oxford handbook on eye movements. Oxford: University Press, pp. 839855.Google Scholar
Duyck, W, Vanderelst, D, Desmet, T and Hartsuiker, RJ (2008) The frequency effect in second–language visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15, 850855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engbert, R, Nuthmann, A, Richter, EM and Kliegl, R (2005) SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review 112, 777813. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotekova, TA and Akhutina, TV (2002) Diagnostika rečevyx narušenij škol'nikov s ispol'zovaniem nejropsixologičeskix metodov [In Russian. Diagnosis of speech disorders by neuropsychological method]. Moscow: Arkti.Google Scholar
Gollan, TH, Montoya, RI, Cera, C and Sandoval, TC (2008) More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language 58, 787814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, TH, Slattery, TJ, Goldenberg, D, Van Assche, E, Duyck, W and Rayner, K (2011) Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: The frequency–lag hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140, 186209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gor, K, Cook, SV, Pandza, NB and Chrabaszcz, A (2018, February). Hearing is not seeing: Is there an auditory advantage and a visual disadvantage in Heritage Speakers compared to late L2 learners?. Paper presented at the Third International Conference on Heritage/Community Languages, Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Häikiö, T, Bertram, R, Hyönä, J and Niemi, P (2009) Development of the letter identity span in reading: Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, 167181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawelka, S, Gagl, B and Wimmer, H (2010) A dual–route perspective on eye movements of dyslexic readers. Cognition 115, 367379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huey, EB (1908) Basic studies on reading. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hyönä, J and Bertram, R (2011) Optimal viewing position effects in reading Finnish. Vision Research 51, 12791287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inhoff, AW and Rayner, K (1986) Parafoveal word processing during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word frequency. Perception & Psychophysics 40, 431439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, HS, Liversedge, SP, Blythe, HI, White, SJ and Rayner, K (2009) Word length and landing position effects during reading in children and adults. Vision Research 49, 20782086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ke, C (1998) Effects of language background on the learning of Chinese characters among foreign language students. Foreign Language Annals 31, 91102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, GD, VanPatten, B and Jegerski, J (2011) Who was walking on the beach?: Anaphora resolution in Spanish heritage speakers and adult second language learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33, 193221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R, Grabner, E, Rolfs, M and Engbert, R (2004) Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 16, 262284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koda, K, Zhang, Y and Yang, CL (2008) Literacy development in Chinese as a heritage language. In He, AW & Xiao, Y (eds), Chinese as a heritage language: Fostering rooted world citizenry. Honolulu: Foreign Language Annals, National Foreign Language Resources Center, pp. 137149.Google Scholar
Korneev, A, Akhutina, T and Matveeva, E (2017) Silent reading in Russian junior schoolchildren: an eye tracking study. Psychology, Journal of Higher School of Economics 14, 219235.Google Scholar
Kuperman, V and Van Dyke, JA (2011) Effects of individual differences in verbal skills on eye–movement patterns during sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language 65, 4273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurinavichyute, AK, Sekerina, IA, Alexeeva, S, Bagdasaryan, K and Kliegl, R (2019) Russian Sentence Corpus: Benchmark measures of eye movements in reading in Russian. Behavior research methods 51, 11611178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X, Liu, P and Rayner, K (2011) Eye movement guidance in Chinese reading: Is there a preferred viewing location?. Vision Research 51, 11461156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liversedge, SP, Drieghe, D, Li, X, Yan, G, Bai, X and Hyönä, J (2016) Universality in eye movements and reading: A trilingual investigation. Cognition 147, 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdecke, D (2017) sjPlot: Data visualization for statistics in social science (R package version 2.3.3). Retrieved from https://CRAN.Rproject.org/package=sjPlotGoogle Scholar
Lyashevskaya, ON and Sharov, SA (2009) Chastotnyj Slovar’ Sovremennogo Russkogo Jazyka (na Materialakh Natsional'nogo Korpusa Russkogo Jazyka) [Frequency Dictionary of Modern Russian (based on the materials of the Russian National Corpus)]. Moscow, Russia: Azbukovnik.Google Scholar
Mancheva, L, Reichle, ED, Lemaire, B, Valdois, S, Ecalle, J and Guérin-Dugué, A (2015) An analysis of reading skill development using E-Z Reader. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 27, 657676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montrul, SA (2008) Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re–examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuthmann, A, Engbert, R and Kliegl, R (2005) Mislocated fixations during reading and the inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision research 45, 22012217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, KB, Almabruk, AA, McGowan, VA, White, SJ and Jordan, TR (2015) Effects of word length on eye movement control: The evidence from Arabic. Psychonomic bulletin & review 22, 14431450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakhlin, N, Kornilov, SA and Grigorenko, EL (2017) Ch. 16 Learning to read Russian. In Verhoeven, L & Perfetti, C (eds), Learning to read across languages and writing systems. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 371392.Google Scholar
O'Regan, JK and Jacobs, AM (1992) Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18, 185197.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M (2006) Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 14, 191262.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M (2018) Heritage languages and their speakers. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M and Kagan, O (2007) Heritage languages: In the ‘wild’ and in the classroom. Language and Linguistics Compass 1, 368395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potowski, K, Jegerski, J and Morgan-Short, K (2009) The effects of instruction on linguistic development in Spanish heritage language speakers. Language Learning 59, 537579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radach, R and Kennedy, A (2004) Theoretical perspectives on eye movements in reading: Past controversies, current issues, and an agenda for future research. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 16, 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2016) R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rayner, K (1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124, 372422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K (2009) Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62, 14571506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichle, ED, Pollatsek, A, Fisher, DL and Rayner, K (1998) Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review 105, 125157. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L and Siyanova–Chanturia, A (2013) Using eye–tracking to investigate topics in L2 acquisition and L2 processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 35, 213235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scontras, G, Fuchs, Z and Polinsky, M (2015) Heritage language and linguistic theory. Frontiers in Psychology 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D (2008) On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin 134, 584615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staub, A, White, SJ, Drieghe, D, Hollway, EC and Rayner, K (2010) Distributional effects of word frequency on eye fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 36, 12801293.Google ScholarPubMed
Staub, A and Rayner, K (2007) Eye movements and on–line comprehension processes. In Gaskell, MG (ed), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 325342.Google Scholar
Tse, L (2001) Heritage language literacy: A study of US biliterates. Language Culture and Curriculum 14, 256268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdés, G (2000) The teaching of heritage languages: An introduction for Slavic–teaching professionals. In Kagan, O & Rifkin, B (eds), The learning and teaching of Slavic languages and cultures. Bloomington, IN: Slavica Publishers, pp. 375403.Google Scholar
Vasishth, S, von der Malsburg, T and Engelmann, P (2013) What eye movements can tell us about sentence comprehension? Wiley Interdisciplinary Review 4, 125134.Google ScholarPubMed
Vitu, F, McConkie, GW, Kerr, P and O'Regan, JK (2001) Fixation location effects on fixation durations during reading: An inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision research 41, 3513-3533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitford, V and Titone, D (2012) Second–language experience modulates first–and second–language word frequency effects: Evidence from eye-movement measures of natural paragraph reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 19, 7380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitford, V and Titone, D (2015) Second-language experience modulates eye movements during first-and second-language sentence reading: Evidence from a gaze-contingent moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41, 11181129.Google ScholarPubMed
Whitford, V and Titone, D (2016) Eye movements and the perceptual span during first–and second–language sentence reading in bilingual older adults. Psychology and Aging 31, 5870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitford, V and Titone, D (2017) The effects of word frequency and word predictability during first–and second–language paragraph reading in bilingual older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging 32, 158177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitford, V and Joanisse, MF (2018) Do eye movements reveal differences between monolingual and bilingual children's first-language and second-language reading? A focus on word frequency effects. Journal of experimental child psychology 173, 318337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, RW (2011) Wookcock reading mastery tests third edition manual. Oxford: Pearson.Google Scholar
Xiao, Y (2006) Heritage learners in the Chinese language classroom: Home background. Heritage Language Journal 4, 4756.Google Scholar
Yarbus, AL (1967) Eye movements and vision. New York, NY: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, H and Koda, K (2018) Vocabulary knowledge and morphological awareness in Chinese as a heritage language (CHL) reading comprehension ability. Reading and Writing 31, 5374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Parshina et al. supplementary material

Tables S5-S15

Download Parshina et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 423 KB