Identifying obstacles is not enough for everybody—Differential efficacy of an intervention fostering fifth graders’ comprehension for word problems

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2020.100953Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Understanding word problems demands comprehension strategies and language awareness.

  • Language-responsive interventions use variation principle and strategic scaffolding.

  • Identifying obstacles is not enough for implementing effective teaching.

  • Effective teaching needs teachers’ professional development and curriculum support.

Abstract

For students’ success in treating word problems, language obstacles have been identified. Interventions are required for students to overcome them, likewise professional development and curriculum support for teachers. But how much support do teachers need? In a controlled trial evaluation study, two interventions for n = 275 fifth graders are compared: The language-responsive intervention (LRI) uses the variation principle and strategic scaffolding fostering comprehension strategies and language awareness. The information-problems-only intervention (IPI) uses the same set of word problems, but teachers were only informed about typical obstacles. For the evaluation study, the ANOVA shows that in both interventions, students have significantly more learning gains than in the no-treatment group, and substantially more in the LRI than in IPI. Students with different language backgrounds profit comparably from LRI, but in different subscales. In conclusion, identifying obstacles is not enough for implementing effective teaching, it should be enhanced by curriculum support.

Introduction

How can innovations be developed and implemented that help students to overcome typical challenges? We focus this major question on word problems, a typical type of tasks that play an important role in mathematics classrooms as they can enhance or assess reality-oriented considerations. The process of reading, understanding and solving mathematical word problems has widely been described, e.g. by Reusser (1997) for arithmetic word problems. Empirical studies contributed to identifying students’ challenges with word problem in various obstacles (Reusser, 1997; Verschaffel, Greer, & de Corte, 2000). Obstacles, defined as potentially challenging features, were shown to occur in various dimensions, among them mathematical, semantical, structural, strategic, conceptual and language obstacles (Daroczy, Wolska, Meurers, & Nuerk, 2015; Reusser, 1997; Verschaffel et al., 2000).

As a consequence of identifying obstacles, effective instructional approaches have been developed and evaluated for mastering conceptual obstacles in word problems (e.g., Van Dooren, De Bock, Hessels, Janssens, & Verschaffel, 2004), for mastering mathematical, structural and semantical obstacles (Rudnitsky, Etheredge, Freeman, & Gilbert, 1995; Xin & Jitendra, 1999; Zheng, Flynn, & Swanson, 2013), and also for strategic and/or habitual obstacles (e.g., Prediger & Krägeloh, 2015; Reusser, 1997; Verschaffel, Greer, Dooren, & Mukhopadhyay, 2010).

This article focuses on language obstacles which comprise e.g. unfamiliar vocabulary or specific meanings of words (e.g. of prepositions or technical terms) on the word level or passive voice and complexe subclauses on the sentence level (Daroczy et al., 2015). So far, empirical findings on language obstacles have been mainly used for avoiding language biases in assessments for students with limited academic language proficiency (e.g. Abedi & Lord, 2001; Haag, Heppt, Roppelt, & Stanat, 2015). Although this reductionist approach is important for fair assessments, it is not sufficient for learning situations, as already Austin and Howson (1979) have argued: “An alternative to simplifying textual material is that of providing children with special reading instruction intended to help them approach and assimilate mathematical vocabulary and phraseology with greater confidence and facility” (p. 173). The call for amplifying approaches has resulted in various approaches for enhancing students’ comprehension strategies (see above), but not many approaches focus on language obstacles on a structural level (as summarized by Erath et al., 2021; Solano-Flores, 2010). Thus, limited empirical evidence exists so far for the efficacy of instructional approaches for fostering students’ competency in overcoming language obstacles.

This article intends to reduce this research gap by investigating the efficacy and implementability of a language-responsive intervention focusing on fifth graders’ strategies and language awareness. The designed teaching-learning arrangement aims at fostering reading and understanding of word problems on two levels of reading processes: (1) on the text level, i.e. fostering subject-specific reading and understanding strategies through strategic scaffolding with concept maps as a strategic scaffold (Hannafin, Land, & Oliver, 1999) and (2) on the sentence level, i.e. fostering syntactic language awareness through the variation principle (Marton & Pang, 2006).

Whenever innovations are implemented in classroom practice, the big question is how much support teachers need for implementing the innovation (Pellegrini, Lake, Inns, & Slavin, 2018; Saxe, Gearhart, & Nasir, 2001). Is it enough to inform the teachers about typical obstacles, or do they also need support in realizing fostering approaches? In our evaluation study, the language-responsive intervention is compared to an information-problems-only intervention (in which teachers were informed about typical language obstacles, but not supported to foster their students in overcoming them), and a no-treatment control group. The teachers’ preparation for both intervention groups is in line with teachers’ demands for the possibilities of direct implementation of PD themes and data into their teaching practice (Wayman & Jimerson, 2014).

The following section introduces the empirical and theoretical background on typical language obstacles, and specifies comprehension strategies and language awareness required for overcoming them. This is the base for justifying the design principles underlying the language-responsive intervention presented in the second section. Its efficacy is investigated by a cluster-randomized controlled trial for which the research design of the evaluation study and the methods are explicated in the third section. The empirical section then presents the differential results for students with high and low general reading proficiency of the study.

Section snippets

Theoretical and empirical backgrounds on language obstacles in word problems

Empirical research on students’ processes of solving arithmetic word problems have identified crucial steps and located typical obstacles in these steps (Plath & Leiß, 2018; Reusser, 1997):

  • (1)

    Understanding of the textbase includes reading competencies on a basic level. Potentially, language obstacles but also habitual obstacles can occur in this step (Prediger, Wilhelm, Büchter, Gürsoy, & Benholz, 2018).

  • (2)

    Understanding of the situation requires generating a mental model of the situation model and a

Language-responsive design principles for fostering comprehension strategies and language awareness for mathematical word problems

As Heid et al. (2006) have emphasized in their strategic position paper, research should not stop with identifying obstacles and specifying learning contents, i.e. components of word problem comprehension, but should bridge the gap to classroom practices by collaboratively developing instructional approaches and concrete teaching units for fostering the learning contents. For language in mathematics classrooms, various teaching approaches are summarized by Erath et al. (2021).

With respect to

Overview

Fig. 5 gives an overview of the design of the cluster-randomized controlled trial with pre- and post-test with n = 275 students and n = 22 teachers of middle schools in urban areas. Two competing interventions served as the independent variable compared to a no-treatment control group. Students’ comprehension of word problems and especially their syntactic language awareness were the dependent variables. Language background (multilingual background and reading proficiency) were considered as

Significantly higher learning gains in the language-responsive intervention

Research question RQ1 asks for the overall efficacy of the interventions compared to each other. The results in the first lines of Table 2 reveal that both intervention groups had highly significant learning gains (Ftime/LRI (1, 117) = 89.00 with p <  0.01 and η2 = 0.20; Ftime/IPI (1, 40) = 13.86 with p <  0.01 and η2 = 0.09). Also the NTG group show significant differences from pre- to post-test (Ftime/NTG (1, 117) = 9.77 with p <  0.01), but these baseline gains have very low effect size (η2

Discussion

Students’ difficulties with word problems are well identified with respect to several obstacles. Whereas effective intervention programs have been developed with respect to conceptual and habitual obstacles (Verschaffel et al., 2000), and sometimes strategic obstacles (e.g., Reusser, 1994; Schukajlow et al., 2015), language obstacles on the word, sentence and text level have been mainly discussed with respect to reductive approaches of simplifying texts.

This paper, in contrast, reports on a

Funding

This study was conducted within the MuM-research group (Mathematics learning under conditions of language diversity) at TU Dortmund University. It was partly funded by the German Ministry for Research and Education (Grant No. 03VP02270), partly by a grant by the German Telekom Foundation, both to S. Prediger. We thank our colleague Henrike Weinert for her statistical support.

References (70)

  • E. Bialystok et al.

    Towards a definition of metalinguistic skill

    Merrill-Palmer Quarterly

    (1985)
  • W. Bos et al.

    Leistungsprofile von Viertklässlerinnen und Viertklässlern

  • R.M. Capraro et al.

    Reading-enhanced word problem solving: A theoretical model

    European Journal of Psychology of Education

    (2012)
  • T.P. Carpenter et al.

    Results and implications of the second NEAP mathematics assessment: Elementary school

    The Arithmetic Teacher

    (1980)
  • A.C.K. Cheung et al.

    How methodological features affect effect sizes in education

    Educational Researcher

    (2016)
  • E.V. Clark

    Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do

  • G. Daroczy et al.

    Word problems: A review of linguistic and numerical factors contributing to their difficulty

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2015)
  • J. Dröse

    Textaufgaben lesen und verstehen lernen. Entwicklungsforschungsstudie zur mathematik spezifischen Leseverständnisförderung [Reading and comprehending word problems – A design research study on mathematics-specific reading comprehension]

    (2019)
  • J. Dröse et al.

    Enhancing Fifth Gradersö Awareness of Syntactic Features in Mathematical Word Problems: A Design Research Study on the Variation Principle

    Journal f’r Mathematik-Didaktik

    (2020)
  • J. Dröse et al.

    Förderbaustein Verstehen von Textaufgaben

  • A. Dyrvold et al.

    Uncommon vocabulary in mathematical tasks in relation to demand of reading ability and solution frequency

    Nordic Studies in Mathematics Education

    (2015)
  • K. Erath et al.

    Designing and enacting instruction that enhances language for mathematics learning - A review of the state of development and research

    ZDM Mathematics Education.

    (2021)
  • N. Förster et al.

    Effects of providing teachers with information about their students’ reading progress

    School Psychology Review

    (2015)
  • P. Gibbons

    Scaffolding language, scaffolding learning. Teaching second language learners in the mainstream classroom

    (2002)
  • A. Gold

    Lesen kann man lernen - Lesestrategien für das 5. und 6. Schuljahr

    (2010)
  • O. Gracía

    Critical multilingual language awareness and teacher education

  • F. Gu et al.

    Theory and development of teaching through variation in mathematics in China

  • N. Haag et al.

    Linguistic simplification of mathematics items: Effects for language minority students in Germany

    European Journal of Psychology of Education

    (2015)
  • M. Hagena et al.

    Using reading strategy training to foster students’ mathematical modelling competencies: Results of a quasi-experimental control trial

    Eurasia Journal of Mathematics Science and Technology Education

    (2017)
  • M. Hannafin et al.

    Open learning environment: Foundations, methods, and models

  • M. Hegarty et al.

    Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: A comparison of successful and unsuccessful problem solvers

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1995)
  • M.K. Heid et al.

    The Enhancing Fifth Graders’ Awarene

    Journal for Research in Mathematics Education

    (2006)
  • F. Hellmich et al.

    Transfereffekte eines wortschatzbasierten Lesestrategietrainings auf die Bearbeitungsqualität mathematischer Textaufgaben bei Grundschülerinnen und -schülern mit Zuwanderungsgeschichte

  • H.C. Hill et al.

    Teacher knowledge, curriculum materials, and quality of instruction: Lessons learned and open issues

    Journal of Curriculum Studies

    (2012)
  • R. Huang et al.

    Teaching and learning mathematics through variation. Confucian heritage meets western theories

    (2017)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text