Skip to main content
Log in

Financial forecasting and risky decisions: an experimental study grounded in Prospect theory

  • Published:
International Entrepreneurship and Management Journal Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study employed prospect theory to examine relationships between effort invested in developing financial forecasts and risk taking. Results of an experimental study indicated that the more effort subjects invested in developing forecasts, the more likely they were to use those forecasts as their reference points when evaluating venture performance. Results also indicated that subjects who used forecasts as their reference points and exerted greater effort developing those forecasts were more likely to take risky actions when performance fell below their reference points. This study is the first to link effort to the type of reference point used and the first to link effort and the use of financial forecasts to risky decisions. In addition, it is one of only a few studies to employ prospect theory to examine risk taking decisions subsequent to start-up. Its results enhance our understanding of risk taking, prospect theory and reference points.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abeler, J., Falk, A., Goette, L., & Huffman, D. (2011). Reference points and effort provision. The American Economic Review, 101(2), 470–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aguinis, H., & Lawal, S. O. (2012). Conducting field experiments using eLancing's natural environment. Journal of Business Venturing, 27(4), 493–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. A. (2004). The cognitive perspective: A valuable tool for answering entrepreneurship's basic "why" questions. Journal of Business Venturing, 19(2), 221–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. J., Margolis, M. F., & Sances, M. W. (2016). Can we turn shirkers into workers? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66(9), 20–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinckmann, J., Grichnik, D., & Kapsa, D. (2010). Should entrepreneurs plan or just storm the castle? A meta-analysis on contextual factors impacting the business planning-performance relationship in small firms. Journal of Business Venturing, 25(1), 24–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, M., Falk, A., & Fehr, E. (2004). Relational contracts and the nature of market interactions. Econometrica, 72(3), 747–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busenitz, L. W., & Barney, J. B. (1997). Differences between entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: Biases and heuristics in strategic decision-making. Journal of Business Venturing, 12, 9–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bushong, B., & Gagnon-Bartsch, T. (2016). Misattribution of reference dependence: Evidence from real-effort experiments. Working Paper: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassar, G. (2010). Are individuals entering self-employment overly optimistic? An empirical test of plans and projections on nascent entrepreneur expectations. Strategic Management Journal, 31(8), 822.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., & Grieco, D. (2019). Creativity and incentives. Journal of European Economics, 17(2), 454–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., Gneezy, U., & Henderson, A. (2018). Experimental methods: Measuring effort in economics experiments. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 149, 74–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, J. H., Burns, D. K., Sinclair, R. R., & Sliter, M. (2017). Amazon mechanical Turk in organizational psychology: An evaluation and practical recommendations. Journal of Business Psychology, 32, 347–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). The design and conduct of true experiments and quasi-experiments in field settings. In R. T. Mowday, & R. M. Steers (Eds.), Reproduced in part in Research in Organizations: Issues and Controversies Goodyear publishing company.

  • Cooper, A. C., Woo, C., & Dunkelberg, W. (1988). Entrepreneurs' perceived chances for success. Journal of Business Venturing, 3(2), 97–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corgnet, B., Hernan-Gonzalez, R., & Rassenti, S. J. (2011). Real effort, real leisure and real-time supervision: Incentives and peer pressure in virtual organizations. Working paper.

  • Corman, J., Perles, B., & Yancini, P. (1988). Motivational factors influencing high-technology entrepreneurship. Journal of Small Business Management, 26(1), 36.

  • Cramer, J. S., Hartog, J., Jonker, N., & Van Praag, C. M. (2002). Low risk aversion encourages the choice for entrepreneurship: An empirical test of a truism. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 48(1), 29–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daly, T. M., & Nataraajan, R. (2015). Swapping bricks for clicks: Crowdsourcing longitudinal data on Amazon Turk. Journal of Business Research, 68, 2603–2609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dambacher, M., Haffke, P., Groß, D., & Hübner, R. (2016). Graphs versus numbers: How information format affects risk aversion in gambling. Judgment and Decision making, 11(3), 223–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estrin, S., Mickiewicz, T., & Rebmann, A. (2017). Prospect theory and the effects of bankruptcy laws on entrepreneurial aspirations. Small Business Economics, 48(4), 977–997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faravelli, M., Kalayci, K., & Pimienta, C. (2019). Costly voting: A large-scale real effort experiment. Experimental Economics, 23, 468–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hack, A., von Bieberstein, F., & Kraiczy, N. D. (2016). Reference point formation and new venture creation. Small Business Economics, 46(3), 447–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hills, G. E. (1985). Market analysis in the business plan: Venture capitalists' perceptions. Journal of Small Business Management, 23(1), 38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hills, G.E. & Shrader, R. C. (1998). Successful entrepreneurs’ insights into opportunity recognition. Frontiers of Entrepreneurship Research, Babson College: 30–43.

  • Hmieleski, K., & Baron, R. (2009). Entrepreneurs' optimism and new venture performance: A social cognitive perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 52(3), 473–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, D. K., Wiklund, J., & Cotton, R. D. (2017). Success, failure, and entrepreneurial reentry: An experimental assessment of the veracity of self-efficacy and prospect theory. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 41(1), 19–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hung, K., & Tangpong, C. (2010). General risk propensity in multifaceted business decisions: Scale development. Journal of Managerial Issues, 22(1), 88–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hvide, H. K., & Panos, G. A. (2014). Risk tolerance and entrepreneurship. Journal of Financial Economics, 111(1), 200–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: A cognitive perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39(1), 17–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80(4), 237–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, F. H. (1921). Risk, uncertainty, and profit. In Boston MA: Hart, Schaffner and Marx. Mifflin: Houghton.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, G., & Bromiley, P. (1997). Decision making in an organizational setting: Cognitive and organizational influences on risk assessment in commercial lending. Academy of Management Journal, 40(5), 1063–1088.

  • Manimala, M. J. (1992). Entrepreneurial heuristics: A comparison between high PL (pioneering-innovative) and low PI ventures. Journal of Business Venturing, 7(6), 477–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Sanchez, V., Contín-Pilart, I., & Larraza-Kintana, M. (2018). The influence of entrepreneurs’ social referents on start-up size. International Entrepreneurship and Management Journal, 14(1), 173–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohnen, A., Pokorny, K., & Sliwka, D. (2008). Transparency, inequity aversion, and the dynamics of peer pressure in teams: Theory and evidence. Journal of Labor Economics, 26(4), 693–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, D. (2007). Experienced utility: Utility theory from Jeremy Bentham to Daniel Kahneman. Thinking and Reasoning, 1(13), 45–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M., & Houghton, S. M. (2003). The relationship between overconfidence and the introduction of risky products: Evidence from a field study. Academy of Management Journal, 46(2), 139–149.

  • Simon, M., & Kim, J. (2017). Two sources of overconfidence: Incorporating disconfirming feedback in an entrepreneurial context. Journal of Small Business Strategy, 27(3), 9–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M., Houghton, S. M., & Aquino, K. (2000). Cognitive biases, risk perception and venture formation: How individuals decide to start companies. Journal of Business Venturing, 15(2), 113–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M., Houghton, S., & Savelli, S. (2003). Out of the frying pan…? Why small business managers introduce high risk products. Journal of Business Venturing, 18(3), 419–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M., Stanton, S. J., Townsend, J. D., & Kim, J. (2019). A multi-method study of social ties and crowdfunding success: Opening the black box to get the cash inside. Journal of Business Research, 104(11), 206–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, S. J., Reeck, C., Huettel, S. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2014). Effects of induced moods on economic choices. Judgment and Decision making, 9(2), 167–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, E. C., & Shu Data, S. B. (2017). Is broad bracketing always better? How broad decision framing leads to more optimal preferences over repeated gambles. Judgment and Decision making, 12(4), 382–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T. (1983). Language as a cognitive process. Reading: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rodney C. Shrader.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shrader, R.C., Simon, M. & Stanton, S. Financial forecasting and risky decisions: an experimental study grounded in Prospect theory. Int Entrep Manag J 17, 1827–1841 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11365-020-00697-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11365-020-00697-4

Keywords

Navigation