Skip to main content
Log in

Effortful Control Development in the Face of Harshness and Unpredictability

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using psychosocial acceleration theory, this multimethod, multi-reporter study examines how early adversity adaptively shapes the development of a self-regulation construct: effortful control. Investigation of links between early life harshness and unpredictability and the development of effortful control could facilitate a nuanced understanding of early environmental effects on cognitive and social development. Using the Building Strong Families national longitudinal data set, aspects of early environmental harshness and early environmental unpredictability were tested as unique predictors of effortful control at age 3 using multiple regression. Early harshness variables were financial harshness, mothers’ and fathers’ observed parenting, mothers’ and fathers’ reported use of harsh discipline, and harsh neighborhood conditions. Early unpredictability was measured by number of paternal transitions. Cues of harshness, specifically observed unresponsive parenting, observed harsh parenting, and neighborhood harshness, did significantly negatively predict effortful control. Paternal transitions also significantly predicted effortful control, but in the opposite (i.e., positive) direction. The results corroborate previous research linking quality of parenting to the development of children’s effortful control and place it within an evolutionary-developmental theoretical framework. Further, the results suggest that neighborhood harshness may also direct developmental trajectories of effortful control in young children, though the mechanisms through which this occurs are still unclear. This is the first study to explicitly investigate effortful control development in early childhood within the harshness and unpredictability framework.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11(1), 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L., & Ellis, B. J. (2012). Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 662–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62(4), 647–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C. (2010). Stress and the development of self-regulation in context. Child Development Perspectives, 4(3), 181–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Individual development and evolution: Experiential canalization of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 647–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, B. L., & O'Connell, R. T. (1990). Linear statistical models: An applied approach. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2014). Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 34(3), 225–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., & Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: Effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion, 13(1), 47–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brumbach, B. H., Figueredo, A. J., & Ellis, B. J. (2009). Effects of harsh and unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life history strategies: A longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Human Nature, 20(1), 25–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza de Baca, T., & Ellis, B. J. (2017). Early stress, parental motivation, and reproductive decision-making: Applications of life history theory to parental behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza de Baca, T., Barnett, M. A., & Ellis, B. J. (2016). The development of the child unpredictability schema: Regulation through maternal life history trade-offs. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 10(1), 43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J. D., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2007). Fathers' influence on their children's cognitive and emotional development: From toddlers to pre-K. Applied Developmental Science, 11(4), 208–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chisholm, J. S. (1999). Death, hope and sex: Steps to an evolutionary ecology of mind and morality. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copping, L. T., Campbell, A., & Mincer, S. (2014). Conceptualizing time preference: A life-history analysis. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(4), 829–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M. (2014). An evolutionary life history framework for psychopathology. Psychological Inquiry, 25(3–4), 261–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M. (2015). Self-regulation in an evolutionary perspective. In G. H. E. Gendolla, M. Tops, & S. L. Koole (Eds.), Handbook of biobehavioral approaches to self-regulation (pp. 25–41). New York: Springer Science + Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(7), 1562–1592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In E. Bialystok & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70–95). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., & Del Giudice, M. (2014). Beyond allostatic load: Rethinking the role of stress in regulating human development. Development and Psychopathology, 26(1), 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh vs. unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20(2), 204–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finegood, E. D., & Blair, C. (2017). Poverty, parent stress, and emerging executive functions in young children. In K. Deater-Deckard & R. Panneton (Eds.), Parental stress and early child development: Adaptive and maladaptive outcomes (pp. 181–207). Cham: Springer International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankenhuis, W. E., & de Weerth, C. (2013). Does early-life exposure to stress shape or impair cognition? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 407–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottschall, A. C., West, S. G., & Enders, C. K. (2012). A comparison of item-level and scale-level multiple imputation for questionnaire batteries. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 47(1), 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardaway, C. R., Wilson, M. N., Shaw, D. S., & Dishion, T. J. (2012). Family functioning and externalizing behaviour among low-income children: Self-regulation as a mediator. Infant and Child Development, 21(1), 67–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, S., Sung, S., Simpson, J. A., Schlomer, G. L., & Belsky, J. (2018). Decomposing environmental unpredictability in forecasting adolescent and young adult development: A two-sample study. Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 1321–1332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karoly, P. (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation: A systems view. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 23–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karreman, A., van Tuijl, C., van Aken, M. A. G., & Deković, M. (2008). Parenting, coparenting, and effortful control in preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(1), 30–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kopystynska, O., Paschall, K. W., Barnett, M. A., & Curran, M. A. (2017). Patterns of interparental conflict, parenting, and children’s emotional insecurity: A person-centered approach. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(7), 922–932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L. J. (2012). Poverty, the development of effortful control, and children’s academic, social, and emotional adjustment. In V. Maholmes & R. B. King (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development (pp. 491–511). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L. J., Honorado, E., & Bush, N. R. (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 40–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, Z., Liu, S., Hartman, S., & Belsky, J. (2018). Interactive effects of early-life income harshness and unpredictability on children’s socioemotional and academic functioning in kindergarten and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 54(11), 2101–2112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, M., Riis, J. L., Ghazarian, S. R., & Johnson, S. B. (2017). Income, family context, and self-regulation in 5-year-old children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 38(2), 99–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Razza, R. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2012). Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development. Early Child Development and Care, 182(10), 1247–1263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menard, S. (2002). Applied logistic regression analysis (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences Vol. 106). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • Mittal, C., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people’s childhood environment: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 621–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittal, C., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Sung, S., & Young, E. S. (2015). Cognitive adaptations to stressful environments: When childhood adversity enhances adult executive function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(4), 604–621.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Duration and developmental timing of poverty and children’s cognitive and social development from birth through third grade. Child Development, 76(4), 795–810.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21(2), 387–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Examiners’ manual: Leiter international performance scale, revised. Chicago: Stoelting.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L. T., & Hill, E. M. (2002). Childhood unpredictability, schemas for unpredictability, and risk taking. Social Behavior and Personality, 30(5), 453–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3, 6th ed., pp. 99–166). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (2002). Temperament in children. In C. von Hofsten & L. Bäckman (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of the millennium (Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives) (Vol. 2, pp. 17–35). Florence: Taylor & Frances/Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh, & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner (pp. 167–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 21–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hersey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development, 72(5), 1394–1408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7(2), 147–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2015). Effortful control. In R. A. Scott & S. M. Kosslyn (Eds.), Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 1–11). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan, D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent–child conflict tactics scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(4), 249–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., Hentges, R. F., & Coe, J. L. (2017). Family instability and children's effortful control in the context of poverty: Sometimes a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush. Development and Psychopathology, 29(3), 685–696.

    Google Scholar 

  • US Census Bureau. (2013). How census measures poverty. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/visualizations/2014/demo/poverty_measure-how.pdf

  • US Department of Health and Human Services. (2008). Federal register, 73(15) (FR Doc. 08–256), pp. 3971–3972. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.

  • van Ginkel, J. R., Linting, M., Rippe, R. C. A., & van der Voort, A. (2019). Rebutting existing misconceptions about multiple imputation as a method for handling missing data. Journal of Personality Assessment. https://doi-org.ezproxy2.library.arizona.edu/10.1080/00223891.2018.1530680.

  • Wenner, C. J., Bianchi, J., Figueredo, A. J., Rushton, J. P., & Jacobs, W. J. (2013). Life history theory and social deviance: The mediating role of executive function. Intelligence, 41(2), 102–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, R. G., Moore, Q., Clarkwest, A., Killewald, A., & Monahan, S. (2012). The long-term effects of building strong families: A relationship skills education program for unmarried parents (OPRE report # 2012-28A). Washington, DC: Office of Planning, research and evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services (OPRE).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, Q., Chen, S. H., & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 112–121.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We want to thank Tomàs Cabeza de Baca for his helpful feedback on a previous version of this manuscript. Preliminary results from this research were presented during the 2018 National Research Conference on Early Childhood in a poster titled “Early Life Harshness & Unpredictability: Adaptively Shaping Young Children’s Effortful Control.”

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shannon M. Warren.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Warren, S.M., Barnett, M.A. Effortful Control Development in the Face of Harshness and Unpredictability. Hum Nat 31, 68–87 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-019-09360-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-019-09360-6

Keywords

Navigation