Skip to main content
Log in

No Association between 2D:4D Ratio and Hunting Success among Hadza Hunters

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ratio of index- and ring-finger lengths (2D:4D ratio) is thought to be related to prenatal androgen exposure, and in many, though not all, populations, men have a lower average digit ratio than do women. In many studies an inverse relationship has been observed, among both men and women, between 2D:4D ratio and measures of athletic ability. It has been further suggested that, in hunter-gatherer populations, 2D:4D ratio might also be negatively correlated with hunting ability, itself assumed to be contingent on athleticism. This hypothesis has been tested using endurance running performance among runners from a Western, educated, and industrialized population as a proximate measure of hunting ability. However, it has not previously been tested among actual hunter-gatherers using more ecologically valid measures of hunting ability and success. The current study addresses this question among Tanzanian Hadza hunter-gatherers. I employ a novel method of assessing hunting reputation that, unlike previous methods, allows granular distinctions to be made between hunters at all levels of perceived ability. I find no statistically significant relationship between digit ratio and either hunting reputation or two important hunting skills. I confirm that Hadza men have higher mean 2D:4D ratios than men in many Western populations. I discuss the notion that 2D:4D ratio may be the consequence of an allometric scaling relationship between relative and absolute finger lengths. Although it is difficult to draw clear conclusions from these results, the current study provides no support for the theorized relationship between 2D:4D ratio and hunting skill.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Statistics from a study of 849 Californian men published by Lippa (2003:183), one of the few studies to report absolute finger lengths.

References

  • Apicella, C. L. (2014). Upper-body strength predicts hunting reputation and reproductive success in Hadza hunter-gatherers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(6), 508–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apicella, C. L., Tobolsky, V. A., Marlowe, F. W., & Miller, K. W. (2016). Hadza hunter-gatherer men do not have more masculine digit ratios (2D:4D). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 159(2), 223–232.

  • Apicella, C. L., Crittenden, A. N., & Tobolsky, V. A. (2017). Hunter-gatherer males are more risk-seeking than females, even in late childhood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 592–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, M., Manning, J. T., Cook, C. J., & Kilduff, L. P. (2010). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and performance in elite rugby players. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28(13), 1415–1421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berbesque, J. C., Wood, B. M., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, A., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016). Eat first, share later: Hadza hunter-gatherer men consume more while foraging than in central places. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(4), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2008). Why women hunt: Risk and contemporary foraging in a Western Desert Aboriginal community. Current Anthropology, 49(4), 655–693.

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (2016). Demography and evolutionary ecology of Hadza hunter-gatherers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., & Marlowe, F. W. (2002). Selection for delayed maturity. Human Nature, 13(2), 199–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., Hawkes, K., & O’Connell, J. F. (2002). Antiquity of postreproductive life: Are there modern impacts on hunter-gatherer postreproductive life spans? American Journal of Human Biology, 14(2), 184–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M. J. (2008). Digit ratio as an indicator of numeracy relative to literacy in 7-year-old British schoolchildren. British Journal of Psychology, 99(1), 75–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. M., Finn, C. J., & Breedlove, S. M. (2002a). Sexual dimorphism in digit-length ratios of laboratory mice. Anatomical Record, 267(3), 231–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. M., Hines, M., Fane, B. A., & Breedlove, S. M. (2002b). Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 42(4), 380–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. S., Marean, C. W., Jacobs, Z., Schoville, B. J., Oestmo, S., Fisher, E. C., Bernatchez, J., Karkanas, P., & Matthews, T. (2012). An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa. Nature, 491(7425), 590–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burley, N. T., & Foster, V. S. (2004). Digit ratio varies with sex, egg order and strength of mate preference in zebra finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271(1536), 239–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M. L., Burkova, V., & Mabulla, A. Z. (2010). Sex differences in 2D:4D ratio, aggression and conflict resolution in African children and adolescents: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 2(1), 17–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M. L., Vasilyev, V. A., Lazebny, O. E., Burkova, V. N., Kulikov, A. M., Mabulla, A., Shibalev, D. V., & Ryskov, A. P. (2012). Aggression, digit ratio, and variation in the androgen receptor, serotonin transporter, and dopamine D4 receptor genes in African foragers: The Hadza. Behavior Genetics, 42(4), 647–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butovskaya, M., Burkova, V., Karelin, D., & Fink, B. (2015). Digit ratio (2D:4D), aggression, and dominance in the Hadza and the Datoga of Tanzania. American Journal of Human Biology, 27(5), 620–627.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. E., Bergeon Burns, C. M., & Ketterson, E. D. (2013). Testosterone production, sexually dimorphic morphology, and digit ratio in the dark-eyed junco. Behavioral Ecology, 24(2), 462–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 299–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Direnzo, G. V., & Stynoski, J. L. (2012). Patterns of second-to-fourth digit length ratios (2D:4D) in two species of frogs and two species of lizards at La Selva, Costa Rica. Anatomical Record, 295(4), 597–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Wlodarski, R. (2016). Reply to Falcon. Biology Letters, 12, 3–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyble, M., Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Page, A., Smith, D., Thompson, J., Vinicius, L., Mace, R., & Migliano, A. B. (2015). Sex equality can explain the unique social structure of hunter-gatherer bands. Science, 348(6236), 769–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falcon, R. G. (2016). Stay, stray or something in-between? A comment on Wlodarski et al. Biology Letters, 12, 1–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forstmeier, W. (2011). Women have relatively larger brains than men: A comment on the misuse of general linear models in the study of sexual dimorphism. The Anatomical Record, 294, 1856–1863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forstmeier, W. (2018). Avoiding misinterpretation of regression lines in allometry: Is sexual dimorphism in digit ratio spurious? bioRxiv, 298786. https://doi.org/10.1101/298786.

  • Froehle, A. W., Wells, G. K., Pollom, T. R., Mabulla, A. Z., Lew-Levy, S., & Crittenden, A. N. (2018). Physical activity and time budgets of Hadza forager children: Implications for self-provisioning and the ontogeny of the sexual division of labor. American Journal of Human Biology, 31(1), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galis, F., Ten Broek, C. M. A., Van Dongen, S., & Wijnaendts, L. C. D. (2010). Sexual dimorphism in the prenatal digit ratio (2D:4D). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(1), 57–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., & Bird, R. B. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 58–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1991). Hunting income patterns among the Hadza: Big game, common goods, foraging goals and the evolution of the human diet. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 334(1270), 243–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2014). More lessons from the Hadza about men’s work. Human Nature, 25(4), 596–619.

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2018). Hunter-gatherer studies and human evolution: A very selective review. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 165(4), 777–800.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in South American hunter-gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1674), 3863–3870.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hönekopp, J., & Watson, S. (2010). Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand. American Journal of Human Biology, 22(5), 619–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hönekopp, J., Voracek, M., & Manning, J. T. (2006). 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and number of sex partners: Evidence for effects of prenatal testosterone in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(1), 30–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hönekopp, J., Bartholdt, L., Beier, L., & Liebert, A. (2007). Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: New data and a meta-analytic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(4), 313–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howlett, C., Setchell, J. M., Hill, R. A., & Barton, R. A. (2015). The 2D:4D digit ratio and social behaviour in wild female chacma baboons in relation to dominance, aggression, interest in infants, affiliation and heritability. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 69, 61–74.

  • Kemper, C. J., & Schwerdtfeger, A. (2009). Comparing indirect methods of digit ratio (2D:4D) measurement. American Journal of Human Biology, 191, 188–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koster, J., McElreath, R., Hill, K., Yu, D., Glenn, S. J., Van Vliet, N., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., Trumble, B., Bliege Bird, R., et al. (2019). The life history of human foraging: Cross-cultural and individual variation. bioRxiv, 574483. https://doi.org/10.1101/574483.

  • Kratochvíl, L., & Flegr, J. (2009). Differences in the 2nd to 4th digit length ratio in humans reflect shifts along the common allometric line. Biology Letters, 5(5), 643–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. (2019). Gender-typed and gender-segregated play among Tanzanian Hadza and Congolese BaYaka hunter-gatherer children and adolescents. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13306.

  • Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2007a). The architecture of human kin detection. Nature, 445(7129), 727–731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D. E., Bramble, D. M., Raichlen, D. A., & Shea, J. J. (2007b). The evolution of endurance running and the tyranny of ethnography: A reply to Pickering and Bunn (2007). Journal of Human Evolution, 53(4), 439–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (2003). Are 2D:4D finger-length ratios related to sexual orientation? Yes for men, no for women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 179–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lolli, L., Batterham, A. M., Kratochvíl, L., Flegr, J., Weston, K. L., & Atkinson, G. (2017). A comprehensive allometric analysis of 2nd digit length to 4th digit length in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 248, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardo, M. P., Otieno, S., & Heiss, A. (2018). College-aged women in the United States that play overhand throwing sports have masculine digit ratios. PLoS One, 13(9), e0203685. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longman, D., Stock, J. T., & Wells, J. C. K. (2011). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and rowing ergometer performance in males and females. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 144(3), 337–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longman, D., Wells, J. C. K., & Stock, J. T. (2015). Can persistence hunting signal male quality? A test considering digit ratio in endurance athletes. PLoS One, 10(4), e0121560. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T. (2002). The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length and performance in skiing. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 42(4), 446–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., & Fink, B. (2008). Digit ratio (2D:4D), dominance, reproductive success, asymmetry, and sociosexuality in the BBC internet study. American Journal of Human Biology, 20(4), 451–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., & Fink, B. (2018). Sexual dimorphism in the ontogeny of second (2D) and fourth (4D) digit lengths, and digit ratio (2D:4D). American Journal of Human Biology, 30, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., & Hill, M. R. (2009). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and sprinting speed in boys. American Journal of Human Biology, 21(2), 210–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., & Taylor, R. P. (2001). Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: Implications for sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(1), 61–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., Wilson, J., & Lewis-Jones, D. I. (1998). The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: A predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Human Reproduction, 13(11), 3000–3004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., Barley, L., Walton, J., Lewis-Jones, D. I., & Trivers, R. L. (2000). The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success: Evidence for sexually antagonistic genes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 163–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., Morris, L., & Caswell, N. (2007). Endurance running and digit ratio (2D:4D): Implications for fetal testosterone effects on running speed and vascular health. American Journal of Human Biology, 19, 416–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2000). The patriarch hypothesis. Human Nature, 11(1), 27–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2004a). Marital residence among foragers. Current Anthropology, 45(2), 277–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2004b). Mate preferences among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Human Nature, 15(4), 365–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Hunting and gathering: The human sexual division of foraging labor. Cross-Cultural Research, 41(2), 170–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Los Angeles: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, M. N., Marlowe, F. W., Bugumba, R., & Ellison, P. T. (2009). Testosterone and paternal care in East African foragers and pastoralists. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1655), 347–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, E., Hoffman, C. L., Gerald, M. S., & Shultz, S. (2010). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and dominance rank in female rhesus macaques. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(6), 1001–1009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oświecimska, J. M., Ksiazek, A., Sygulla, K., Pyś-Spychała, M., Roczniak, G. R., Roczniak, W., Stojewska, M., & Ziora, K. (2012). Androgens concentrations and second to fourth-digit ratio (2D:4D) in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (21-hydroxylase deficiency). Neuroendocrinology Letters, 33(8), 787–791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, S. N., Kato, B. S., Hunkin, J. L., Vivekanandan, S., & Spector, T. D. (2006). The big finger: The second to fourth digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40(12), 981–983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, J. D., Bhagwandin, A., Spocter, M. A., Davimes, J., & Manger, P. R. (2017). Hands of living San resemble those in Palaeolithic stencils, not modern Europeans. Transactions of the Royal Society of South Africa, 73(1), 1–7.

  • Pickering, T. R., & Bunn, H. T. (2007). The endurance running hypothesis and hunting and scavenging in savanna-woodlands. Journal of Human Evolution, 53(4), 434–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., Sporter, R. J., & McBurney, D. H. (2004). Sex hormones and finger length: - what does 2D:4D indicate? Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(3), 182–199.

  • Rammsayer, T. H., & Troche, S. J. (2007). Sexual dimorphism in second-to-fourth digit ratio and its relation to gender-role orientation in males and females. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 911–920.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ribeiro, E., Neave, N., Morais, R. N., & Manning, J. T. (2016). Direct versus indirect measurement of digit ratio (2D:4D): A critical review of the literature and new data. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(1), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Boothroyd, L. G., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Do men’s faces really signal heritable immunocompetence? Behavioural Ecology, 24(3), 579–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., & Marlowe, F. W. (2009). How universal are human mate choices? Size does not matter when Hadza foragers are choosing a mate. Biology Letters, 5(5), 606–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. M., & Apicella, C. L. (2019). Partner choice in human evolution: The role of character, hunting ability, and reciprocity in Hadza campmate selection. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/35tch.

  • Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E. (2019). Costly signaling and the handicap principle in hunter-gatherer research: A critical review. Evolutionary Anthropology, 28, 144–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Attenborough, R. D., & Marlowe, F. W. (2018). A noisy signal: To what extent are Hadza hunting reputations predictive of actual hunting skills? Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(6), 639–651.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobler, M., Healey, M., & Olsson, M. (2011). Digit ratio, color polymorphism and egg testosterone in the Australian painted dragon. PLoS One, 6(1), e16225. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R., Manning, J., & Jacobson, A. (2006). A longitudinal study of digit ratio (2D:4D) and other finger ratios in Jamaican children. Hormones and Behavior, 49(2), 150–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2008). The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(6), 402–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voracek, M., Manning, J. T., & Dressler, S. G. (2007). Repeatability and interobserver error of digit ratio (2D: 4D) measurements made by experts. American Journal of Human Biology, 19(1), 142–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voracek, M., Reimer, B., & Dressler, S. G. (2010). Digit ratio (2D:4D) predicts sporting success among female fencers independent from physical, experience, and personality factors. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, 20(6), 853–860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L., Trower, G., Backwell, L., & D’Errico, F. (2015). Traditional glue, adhesive and poison used for composite weapons by Ju/’hoan San in Nyae Nyae, Namibia. Implications for the evolution of hunting equipment in prehistory. PLoS One, 10(10), e0140269. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkie, D. S., & Curran, B. (1991). Why do Mbuti hunters use nets? Ungulate hunting efficiency of archers and net-hunters in the Ituri rain forest. American Anthropologist, 93(3), 680–689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wlodarski, R., Manning, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). Stay or stray? Evidence for alternative mating strategy phenotypes in both men and women. Biology Letters, 11(2), 20140977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. M. (2006). Prestige or provisioning? A test of foraging goals among the Hadza. Current Anthropology, 47(2), 383–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. M., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Household and kin provisioning by Hadza men. Human Nature, 24(3), 280–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. M., & Marlowe, F. W. (2014). Toward a reality-based understanding of Hadza men’s work. Human Nature, 25(4), 620–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worthman, C. M., & Konner, M. J. (1987). Testosterone levels change with subsistence hunting effort in !Kung San men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 12(6), 449–458.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Thanks to all those who have provided support throughout this project. Special thanks to Frank Marlowe, Robert Attenborough, Jeremy Kendall, Coren Apicella, Sally Street, Enrico Crema, Trevor Hawkes, and Charles Endeko. Thanks also to all those who participated in the project. Further thanks to Bernhard Fink, Martin Voracek, and one anonymous reviewer for the help they have provided in revising and improving this manuscript.

This project was funded with generous support from the Leakey Foundation, Robinson College Cambridge, the Smuts Memorial Fund, the Ruggles-Gates Fund of the Royal Anthropological Institute, the Cambridge Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, the Cambridge Centre for African Studies, the Anthony Wilkin Fund, and the Ridgeway-Venn Fund.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Duncan N. E. Stibbard-Hawkes.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 458 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stibbard-Hawkes, D.N.E. No Association between 2D:4D Ratio and Hunting Success among Hadza Hunters. Hum Nat 31, 22–42 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-019-09359-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-019-09359-z

Keywords

Navigation