Skip to main content
Log in

Peer Reactions, Peer Behavior, Student Attitudes, and Student Deviance: a Comparison of College Students in Japan and the USA

  • Published:
Asian Journal of Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study presents evidence on the cross-cultural generalizability of differential association/social learning theory by testing whether the causal processes of learning attitudes toward deviance, posited by the theory, are equally applicable, and the causal links, specified by the theory, are equally strong in two diverse cultures—the USA and Japan. Drawing on the literature concerning cultural variability in individualism-collectivism, we predicted that the effects of peer reactions to deviance and peer deviance on a person’s attitudes toward deviance should be stronger in Japan than in the USA, and that the mediating effect of a person’s attitudes on the relationship of peer reactions and peer deviance to a person’s deviance should be weaker among Japanese than among Americans. Analyses of comparable survey data from college students in the USA (N = 625) and Japan (N = 591) provide generally supportive, but somewhat mixed, evidence regarding our predictions. In both countries, peer reactions to deviance predicted student attitudes toward deviance more strongly than did peer deviance. Peer deviance strongly predicted student deviance, while peer reactions to deviance predicted less strongly, and the effects were mediated by student attitudes in both countries. Contrary to the hypotheses, peer reactions and peer deviance did not predict student attitudes more strongly in Japan than in the USA. Also, peer deviance predicted student deviance more strongly in the USA than in Japan. In agreement with the expectations, the relation between student attitudes and student deviance was stronger in the USA than in Japan.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Space does not permit an assessment of the status of the field of “comparative criminology.” For a concise summary, see Dussich et al. (2001, pp. 155–59; see also Vazsonyi 2003).

  2. Scholars argued that, along with modern capitalism and participation in the global economy, Japanese society and Japanese people have become more individualistic and will continue to move in this direction (e.g., Hashimoto and Traphagan 2008; Iwao 1993; Kerbo and McKinstry 1997). While this might be true and might be the long-term trend in Japan, our own data revealed a lower score among the Japanese students than among the Americans on our measure of individualistic value orientation. The difference was not only significant (p < .001) but also substantial (beta = .532) with controls for gender, age, two-adult home, and parent education.

  3. We must emphasize that the present research is not intended as a test of the full model of social learning theory. Rather, it is concentrated on the relationships among the perceived reactions and behavior of a person’s primary groups (i.e., peers) and the person’s attitudes and behavior, without positing that peer reactions and peer behavior are the only or the main sources of group influence.

  4. College undergraduate students were chosen as students for two reasons. First, we had easier access to them than to younger adolescents. Second, college undergraduate students, especially early in their academic years, typically are in their deviance-prone years and, in addition, the questions asked referred retrospectively to their high school days. We realize, of course, that people who did not attend college were excluded from our research design and might be more (or less) deviant than those who did attend college. But the inclusion of only college students applied to both samples.

  5. Japanese students must declare a major before their admission to a university. Unlike universities in the USA, there is no equivalent in Japan to general education courses taken by such a large number of students outside their major.

  6. Some might question about the equivalence of self-report data in the two countries because self-disclosure in Japan, relative to the USA, is usually (and often exaggeratedly) considered to be an inappropriate behavior (Gudykunst and Nishida 1994). However, we made every effort to overcome the problem by informing the Japanese students that the name of the university would never be revealed and that there was no right or wrong answer to any of the questions.

  7. We had to address the wide discrepancy between Japanese national and American state universities in racial and ethnic diversity, a discrepancy so wide that “minority group” status could not be a variable in our analyses. Race/ethnicity is included as a control in tests of deviance theories in the USA. We knew in advance, however, that this would be problematic in our research because of the racial and ethnic homogeneity of Japan. Whereas 77% of the US population is white (U.S. Census 2016b), typical estimates are that 98% in the Japanese population is racially and ethnically Japanese (Statistics Bureau 2017; see also Sugimoto 2003). Consequently, our plan was to use only the questionnaires completed by Caucasian students in the USA, excluding those who self-identified themselves as minority group members. Likewise, we would omit from the analyses the few Japanese students who identified themselves as “non-Japanese.”

  8. In fact, 98% of the students in the Japanese sample were 20 years old or younger and the comparable figure in the US sample was 80%.

  9. A large number of missing cases in the Japanese sample probably can be attributed to their lack of experience in answering a long list of survey questions and the absence of extra credits that could be earned in exchange for their participation in the survey.

  10. The gender composition of universities was another issue to be addressed. In the American university only about half (46%) of all students were male, a figure typical of state universities in the USA. In contrast, Japanese national universities are overwhelmingly male. According to figures from the Japan Association of National Universities (2017), 69% of all students enrolled in all national universities are male. In the particular Japanese university from which we gathered data, 68% of all students enrolled were male. While our American sample reflects this distribution, the lower proportion of males in the Japanese sample indicates a greater unwillingness among Japanese male students to comply with requests to participate in this research.

  11. Similar conclusions were reached with principal component analyses of the dichotomized items within nations (coded 0 for students who “never” engaged in the behavior during high school years and 1 for those who did). For both nations, the scree tests (Cattell 1966) indicated a single factor, and no major differences in factor loadings were obvious between nations. The Alphas for the sum of dichotomies were .68 in both the USA and Japan.

  12. We could not use family income as another indicator of family socio-economic status because we knew, in advance, that a high percentage of college students would not provide an answer. In our previous cross-cultural survey, students were asked to choose one of seven income categories ranging from “less than $15,000” to “$100,000 or more” which were also converted to yen on the Japanese questionnaire. However, a high percentage of American students (12%) did not provide an answer. The figure was even higher—38%—for Japanese students. The greater non-response rate for the Japanese probably could be attributed to their greater reluctance to provide such information and the greater likelihood of not knowing their parent income. Consequently, parent income was not included in the present questionnaire and parent education was used as the indicator of family socio-economic status.

  13. Some might argue that there are differences in results based on the use of standardized versus unstandardized scores. But we would note that we also ran the analyses of the relationships presented in Tables 3 and 4 using the scales with standardized variables and found that there was no significant difference in results from the two methods.

References

  • Akers, R. L. (1985). Deviant behavior: a social learning approach (3rd ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akers, R. L. (1998). Social learning and social structure: a general theory of crime and deviance. Boston: Northeastern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akers, R. L., Sellers, C. S., & Jennings, W. G. (2017). Criminological theories: introduction, evaluation, and application (7th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aliverdinia, A., & Pridemore, W. A. (2007). A first glimpse at narcotics offenders in an Islamic Republic: a test of an integrated model of drug involvement among a sample of men incarcerated for drug offenses in Iran. Criminal Justice Review, 17, 27–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antonaccio, O., Tittle, C. R., Botchkovar, E., & Kranidioti, M. (2010). The correlates of crime: additional evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 47, 297–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belsley, D. A. (1982). Assessing the presence of harmful collinearity and other forms of weak data through a test for signal-to-noise. Journal of Econometrics, 20, 211–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruinsma, G. J. N. (1992). Differential association theory reconsidered: an extension and its empirical test. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 8, 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, R. L., & Akers, R. L. (1966). A differential association reinforcement theory of criminal behavior. Social Problems, 14, 128–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1, 245–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cernkovich, S., & Giordano, P. (1992). School bonding, race, and delinquency. Criminology, 30, 261–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, B. J., & Vowell, P. R. (1999). Testing control theory and differential association: a reanalysis of the Richmond youth project data. Criminology, 37, 815–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doi, T. (1971). The anatomy of dependence. New York: Kodansha International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dussich, J. P. J., Friday, P. C., Okada, T., Yamagami, A., & Knutdenm, R. (2001). Different responses to violence in Japan and America. Monsey: Criminal Justice Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gudykunst, W. B., & Nishida, T. (1994). Bridging Japanese/North American differences. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K., & Heyman, S. (1996). The influence of cultural individualism-collectivism, self-construals, and individual values on communication styles across cultures. Human Communication Research, 22, 510–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartjen, C. A., & Priyadarsini, S. (2003). Gender, peers, and delinquency: a study of boys and girls in rural France. Youth & Society, 34, 387–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hashimoto, A., & Traphagan, J. W. (2008). Imagined families, lived families: culture and kinship in contemporary Japan. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimer, K. (1997). Socioeconomic status, subcultural definitions, and violent delinquency. Social Forces, 75, 799–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirtenlefner, H., Pauwels, L., & Mesko, G. (2015). Is the criminogenic effect of exposure to peer delinquency dependent on the ability to exercise self-control? Results from three countries. Journal of Criminal Justice, 43, 532–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. H. (1984). Hofstede’s cultural dimensions: an independent validating using Rokeach’s value survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 417–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and organizations: software of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iwao, S. (1993). The Japanese woman: traditional image and changing reality. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerbo, H. R., & McKinstry, J. (1997). Modern Japan: a volume in the comparative societies series. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, E., Akers, R. L., & Sharp, S. F. (2011). Attitude transference and deviant behavior: a comparative study in Japan and the United States. Deviant Behavior, 32, 405–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsueda, R. L. (1982). Testing control theory and differential association: a causal modeling approach. American Sociological Review, 47, 489–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsueda, R. L., & Heimer, K. (1987). Race, family structure, and delinquency: a test of differential association and social control theories. American Sociological Review, 52, 826–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D. (2006). The new Japan: debunking seven cultural stereotypes. Boston: Intercultural Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Juang, L. (2012). Cultural psychology (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meneses, R. A., & Akers, R. L. (2011). A comparison of four general theories of crime and deviance: marijuana use among American and Bolivian university students. International Criminal Justice Review, 21, 333–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Messner, S. F. (2015). When west meets east: generalizing theory and expanding the conceptual toolkit of criminology. Asian Journal of Criminology, 10, 117–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster, R., Brame, R., Mazerolle, P., & Piquero, A. (1998). Using the correct statistical test for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology, 36, 859–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Sellers, C. S., Winfree Jr., L. T., Madensen, T. D., Daigle, L. E., Fearn, N. E., & Gau, J. M. (2010). The empirical status of social learning theory: a meta-analysis. Justice Quarterly, 27, 765–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sengoku, T. (2005). Nihon no joshi chuukousei [Female junior/high school students in Japan]. Tokyo: NHK Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D. A., & Paternoster, R. (1987). The gender gap in theories of deviance: issues and evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 24, 140–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Bureau. (2015). The number of households. The Japanese Bureau of Census. Tokyo, Japan: Statistics Bureau, the Japanese Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications. Retrieved from http://www.stat.go.jp/data/kokusei/2015/kekka/kihon3/pdf/gaiyou.pdf.

  • Statistics Bureau. (2017). The numbers of foreign residents in Japan. The number of foreign residents in Japan. Tokyo, Japan: Japanese Ministry of Justice. Retrieved from http://www.moj.go.jp/content/001238032.pdf

  • Sugimoto, Y. (2003). An introduction to Japanese society (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, E. H. (1947). Principles of criminology (4th ed.). Chicago: J.B. Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Japan Association of National Universities. (2017). Female ratio transition at Japanese national universities. A follow-up research on promotion of gender equality. Retrieved from http://www.janu.jp/gender/201801houkoku_gaiyou.pdf.

  • Thornberry, T. P., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M. D., Farnworth, M., & Jang, S. J. (1994). Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent behavior: a longitudinal test of interactional theory. Criminology, 32, 47–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tittle, C. R. (1980). Sanctions and social deviance: the question of social deterrence. New York: Praeger Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tittle, C. R., & Paternoster, R. (2000). Social deviance and crime: an organizational and theoretical approach. Los Angeles: Roxbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tittle, C., Antonaccio, O., & Botchkovar, E. (2012). Social learning, reinforcement, and criminal probability. Social Forces, 90, 863–890.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1988). Collectivism vs. individualism: a reconceptualization of a basic concept in cross-cultural psychology. In G. Verma & C. Bagley (Eds.), Cross-cultural studies of personality, attitudes and cognition (pp. 60–95). London: Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census. (2016a). The majority of children live with two parents. Census Bureau Reports, November 2016. Washington, DC: USA. Department of Commerce: U.S. Census Bureau. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/newsroom/press-releases/2016/cb16-192.html.

  • U.S. Census. (2016b). Race and Hispanic origin. Census Bureau Reports, July 2016. Washington, DC: USA. Department of Commerce: U.S. Census Bureau. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/US/RHI125216#viewtop.

  • Vazsonyi, A. T. (2003). Comparative criminology: content or simply methodology. In C. L. Britt & M. R. Gottfredson (Eds.), Control theories of crime and delinquency: advances in criminological theory (Vol. 12, pp. 179–211). New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S., & Jensen, G. F. (2003). Explaining delinquency in Taiwan: a test of social learning theory. In R. L. Akers & G. F. Jensen (Eds.), Social learning theory and the explanation of crime: a guide for the new century (pp. 65–83). New Brunswick: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warr, M., & Stafford, M. (1991). The influence of delinquent peers: what they think or what they do? Criminology, 29, 851–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yun, M., & Kim, E. (2015). Illicit drug use among South Korean offenders: assessing the generality of social learning theory. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 59, 1166–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, L., & Messner, S. F. (1995). Family deviance and delinquency in China. Criminology, 33, 359–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Marvin D. Krohn for his comments on the manuscript and his contribution to the data collection in the USA. We are also appreciative of the generosity of Ronald L. Akers in sharing the questionnaire used for the Boys Town Study. Additionally, we are grateful for the suggestions and insights on earlier versions of the manuscript from Harold G. Grasmick.

Funding

The research reported here was supported by the Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (No. 17K04095) from the Japanese Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emiko Kobayashi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Compliance

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table 6 Descriptive statistics for student deviant behavior items and scales

Appendix 2

Table 7 Descriptive statistics for student attitude items and scales

Appendix 3

Table 8 Descriptive statistics for peer reaction items and scales

Appendix 4

Table 9 Descriptive statistics for peer deviant behavior items and scales

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kobayashi, E., Farrington, D.P. & Buchanan, M. Peer Reactions, Peer Behavior, Student Attitudes, and Student Deviance: a Comparison of College Students in Japan and the USA. Asian J Criminol 14, 3–22 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11417-018-9276-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11417-018-9276-y

Keywords

Navigation