Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2pzkn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-12T07:57:28.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Sentence Repetition Task for Early Language Assessment in Spanish

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 October 2020

Natalia Bravo
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain) Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha (Spain)
Miguel Lázaro
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense de Madrid (Spain)
Sonia Mariscal*
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sonia Mariscal. Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación de la Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia. Madrid (Spain). E-mail: smariscal@psi.uned.es

Abstract

Sentence repetition tasks have been widely used in the last years as a diagnostic tool in developmental language disorders. However in Spanish there are few (if any) of these instruments, especially for younger children. In this context, we develop a new Sentence Repetition Task for assessing language (morphosyntactic) abilities of very young Spanish children. A list of 33 sentences of different length and complexity was created and included in the task. A total of 130 typical developing children from 2 to 4 years of age were engaged in a play situation and asked to repeat the sentences. Children’s answers were scored for accuracy at sentence and word level and error analysis at the word level was undertaken. Besides a subsample of 92 children completed a non-word repetition task. First results show its adequacy to children from 2 to 4 years of age, its capacity to discriminate between different developmental levels, and its concurrent validity with the nonword repetition task.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: None.

Funding Statement: This work was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades, Grant number PGC2018-095275-A-I00. Project entitled: Acompañar y promover el desarrollo del lenguaje en el primer ciclo de Educación Infantil: Indicadores multimodales tempranos de los procesos de comunicación y autorregulación.

References

Adams, A. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Communication Disorders, 35(1), 95116. https://doi.org/10.1080/136828200247278Google ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A.-M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(2), 85106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). Working memory and short-term sentence recall in young children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(2), 207220. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440440000005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archibald, L. M. D, & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52(4), 899914. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0099)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Allen, R. J. (2009). Working memory and binding in sentence recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(3), 438456. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiat, S., Armon-Lotem, S., Marinis, T., Polišenská, K., Roy, P., & Seeff-Gabriel, B. (2013). The potential of sentence imitation tasks for assessment of language abilities in sequential bilingual. In V. C. Mueller Gathercole (Ed.), Issues in the Assessment of Bilinguals (pp. 5689). Multilingual Matters. http://doi.org/10.21832/9781783090105-005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2008). Early phonological and sociocognitive skills as predictors of later language and socio communication outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(6), 635645. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01881CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741748. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devescovi, A., & Caselli, M. C. (2007). Sentence repetition as a measure or early grammatical development in Italian. International Journal of Communication Disorders, 42(2), 187208. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601030686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gábor, B., & Lukács, Á. (2012). Early morphological productivity in Hungarian: Evidence from sentence repetition and elicited production. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 411442. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000911000110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavarró, A. (2017). A sentence repetition task for Catalan-speaking typically-developing children and children with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1865. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01865CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klem, M., Melby‐Lervåg, M., Hagtvet, B., Solveig-Alma, H. L, Gustafsson, J.-E., & Hulme, C. (2015). Sentence repetition is a measure of children’s language skill rather than working memory limitation. Developmental Science, 18(1), 146154. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2018). NEPSY–II. Spanish translation. Pearson Clinical & Talent Assessment. (Original work published in 2007).Google Scholar
Lázaro, M., Rujas, I., Montero, I., Murillo, E., & Casla, M. (2018). Validation of a scale for a nonword repetition task to assess lexical development Anales de Psicología, 34, 92100. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.34.1.256731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecrercq, A.-N., Quémart, P., Magis, D., & Maillart, C. (2014). The sentence repetition task: A powerful diagnostic tool for French children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 34233430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, M. P., Karousou, A., Mariscal, S., & Martínez, M. (2005). MacArthur: Inventario de desarrollo comunicativo [MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories]. TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2015). Sentence repetition. In Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, N. (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 95124). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S., & Gallego, C. (2013). La imitación como herramienta para investigar y evaluar el desarrollo lingüístico temprano: Un estudio piloto de repetición de palabras y pseudopalabras [Imitation as a tool to investigate and assess early language development: A pilot study in word and non – word repetition]. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia, 3(1), 5375.Google Scholar
Moreno-Torres, I., Madrid, S. & Moruno, E. (2013). Prueba de repetición de oraciones para niños de 24 a 48 meses (PRO–24). Estudio piloto con niños típicos y niños sordos con implante coclear [Test on sentence repetition for children between 24–48 months. Pilot study with typical developing children and hearing impaired children with cochlear implant]. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 33(1), 2535. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rlfa.2012.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nag, S., Snowling, M., & Mirković, J. (2018). The role of language production mechanisms in children’s sentence repetition: Evidence from an inflectionally rich language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(2), 303325. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D., Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Iglesias, A., Goldstein, B. A., & Bedore, L. M. (2014). BESA. Bilingual English-Spanish Assessment. AR- Clinical Publications.Google Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Genesee, F, Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variation in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(6), 12651300. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polišenská, K., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2015). Sentence repetition: What does the task measure? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 50(1), 106118. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riches, N. G. (2012). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment: An investigation of underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 47(5), 499510. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00158.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roid, G. H., Sampers, J. L., Anderson, G., Erickson, J., & Post, P. (2011). Merrill-Palmer-R. Escalas de Desarrollo [Merrill-Palmer-Revised Scales of Development]. TEA Ediciones. (Original work published in 2004).Google Scholar
Rujas, I., Mariscal, S., Casla, M., Lázaro, M., & Murillo, E. (2017). Word and nonword repetition abilities in Spanish language: Longitudinal evidence from typically developing and late talking children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20, e72. http://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2017.69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szewczyk, J. M., Marecka, M., Chiat, S., & Wodniecka, Z. (2018). Nonword repetition depends on the frequency of sublexical representations at different grain sizes: Evidence from a multi-factorial analysis. Cognition, 179, 2336. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seeff-Gabriel, B., Chiat, S., & Dodd, B. (2010). Sentence imitation as a tool in identifying expressive morphosyntactic difficulties in children with severe speech difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45(6), 691702. http://doi.org/10.3109/13682820903509432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seeff-Gabriel, B., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2008). Early Repetition Battery (ERB). Pearson.Google Scholar
Simón-Cereijido, G. (2017). Sentence repetition in typical and atypical Spanish speaking preeschoolers who are English language learners. In Schwartz, R. G. & Benavides, A. Auza (Eds.), Language development and language disorders in Spanish-speaking children: Language processing and cognitive functions (pp. 205215). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M.-Y., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2006). Nonword repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of specific language impairment: The case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 219236. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thordardottir, E. (2008). Language-specific effects of task demands on the manifestation of specific language impairment: A comparison of English and Icelandic. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(4), 922937. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/068)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (2009). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals- preschool, Spanish edition (CELF–P2 Spanish) (2nd Ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Zebib, R., Tuller, L., Hamann, C., Abed Ibrahim, L., & Prévost, P. (2020). Syntactic complexity and verbal working memory in bilingual children with and without developmental language disorder. First Language, 40(4), 461484. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719888372CrossRefGoogle Scholar