Cross-modal involvement of the primary somatosensory cortex in visual working memory: A repetitive TMS study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107325Get rights and content

Abstract

Recent literature suggests that the primary somatosensory cortex (S1), once thought to be a low-level area only modality-specific, is also involved in higher-level, cross-modal, cognitive functions. In particular, electrophysiological studies have highlighted that the cross-modal activation of this area may also extend to visual Working Memory (WM), being part of a mnemonic network specific for the temporary storage and manipulation of visual information concerning bodies and body-related actions. However, the causal recruitment of S1 in the WM network remains speculation.

In the present study, by taking advantage of repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS), we look for causal evidence that S1 is implicated in the retention of visual stimuli that are salient for this cortical area. To this purpose, in a first experiment, high-frequency (10 Hz) rTMS was delivered over S1 of the right hemisphere, and over two control sites, the right lateral occipital cortex (LOC) and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), during the maintenance phase of a high-load delayed match-to-sample task in which body-related visual stimuli (non-symbolic hand gestures) have to be retained. In a second experiment, the specificity of S1 recruitment was deepened by using a version of the delayed match-to-sample task in which visual stimuli depict geometrical shapes (non-body related stimuli).

Results show that rTMS perturbation of S1 activity leads to an enhancement of participants’ performance that is selective for body-related visual stimuli; instead, the stimulation of the right LOC and dlPFC does not affect the temporary storage of body-related visual stimuli. These findings suggest that S1 may be recruited in visual WM when information to store (and recall) is salient for this area, corroborating models which suggest the existence of a dedicated mnemonic system for body-related information in which also somatosensory cortices play a key role, likely thanks to their cross-modal (visuo-tactile) properties.

Introduction

In the last two decades, the role of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) in high-order cognitive functions has been widely investigated. Studies using different techniques, like functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG) or non-invasive brain stimulation, showed that S1 is activated not only during the elaboration of afferent somatosensory stimuli and vicarious somatosensation by touch observation, but it is also involved in higher-order cognitive functions such as emotion recognition, motor learning by observation and body representation (e.g., Avenanti et al., 2007, Blakemore et al., 2005, Bolognini et al., 2014, Lametti and Watkins, 2016, McGregor et al., 2016, Pisoni et al., 2018, Pitcher et al., 2008, Rossetti et al., 2012, Sel et al., 2014, Valchev et al., 2017, Zazio et al., 2019). The activation of S1 seems to extend also to memory and attention of touch (e.g., Bauer et al., 2006, Forster et al., 2016, Gallace and Spence, 2009, Ku et al., 2015, Papagno et al., 2016, Papagno et al., 2017, Preuschhof et al., 2006, Taylor-Clarke et al., 2002, Zhao and Ku, 2018).

In particular, considering the domain of memory, Christophel and Haynes (2014), in an fMRI experiment, showed that the retention of complex visuo-spatial pattern stimuli (i.e., moving dots), besides activating lower-level visual areas and the posterior parietal cortex, also recruited S1. This result would suggest that visual features may be encoded through a cross-modal (visual-tactile) mapping, a process by which S1 takes part into the storage of visual information in working memory (WM) (Christophel & Haynes, 2014). Other clues on the potential cross-modal recruitment of somatosensory cortical areas in visual WM are provided by the studies of Ku and colleagues (2015, 2017, 2018): these authors showed that participants’ performance in a tactile-visual delayed match-to-sample task was impaired when single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is delivered over S1 300 ms after the onset of the tactile stimulus. Instead, high-order cortical areas, like the posterior parietal cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), were recruited during this cross-modal task at different (longer) timepoints of the mnemonic process. However, in such experiments, the tasks were not purely visual; indeed, the authors adopted cross-modal (tactile-to-visual) WM paradigms requiring to exchange information between touch and vision (Ku et al., 2015, Zhao and Ku, 2018, Zhao et al., 2017).

In addition, there are convergent findings that our brain is equipped with a WM sub-system that is specific for the temporary storage and manipulation of visual information concerning bodies and body-related actions (Galvez-Pol et al., 2020, Rumiati and Tessari, 2002, Shen et al., 2014, Smyth et al., 1988, Smyth and Pendleton, 1989, Wood, 2007). The neural underpinnings of this visual ‘body-related’ memory system comprise a complex network of cerebral areas including specialized visual regions, such as the fusiform gyrus and the extrastriate body area (Peelen and Downing, 2005, Urgesi et al., 2007), premotor and tempo-parietal regions related to the action observation network (Cai et al., 2018, Liu et al., 2019, Lu et al., 2016), but also ‘low-level’ somatosensory and motor areas (Arslanova et al., 2019, Galvez-Pol et al., 2018, Galvez-Pol et al., 2018). Overall, this memory network strongly overlaps the ones implicated in the processing of our own body and motor representations (Longo et al., 2010, Molenberghs et al., 2012). The role of S1 in this WM sub-system has been recently highlighted in an EEG study showing that contralateral delayed activity (CDA), a neurophysiological marker of WM (for a review, see: Luria, Balaban, Awh, & Vogel, 2016), increased in left and right somatosensory cortices selectively during the maintenance of visual body-related stimuli (i.e., non-symbolic hand gestures); moreover, the magnitude of this increase was correlated with the number of stimuli to memorize (Galvez-Pol, Calvo-Merino, et al., 2018).

Despite these recent findings, no study has investigated the causal involvement of S1 in visual WM so far. To this aim, we have run a TMS study to obtain the first causal evidence of the actual recruitment of S1 during a visual body-related WM task. Indeed, as repetitive TMS (rTMS) allows to prove the causal relationship between the stimulated cortical area and its involvement in a cognitive function (Bolognini & Ro, 2010), we applied this technique ‘online’ to interfere with S1 functioning while participants performed a WM task, i.e., delayed match-to-sample task (e.g., Galvez-Pol et al., 2018, Vogel and Machizawa, 2004, Vogel et al., 2005) where the visual stimuli to memorize depicted body-related information (hand gestures – Experiment 1). If S1 is recruited for processing bodily stimuli during visual WM tasks, then the perturbation of its activity should affect participants’ performance when rTMS is delivered at a timepoint corresponding to the maintenance phase of the mnemonic process. Following the evidence that higher memory loads lead to a greater CDA in the electrodes overlapping somatosensory cortices (Galvez-Pol, Calvo-Merino, et al., 2018), we decided to use an experimental task with a high number of visual body-related stimuli to memorize (Luck & Vogel, 2013). We chose S1 of the right hemisphere, following Baddeley’s model (2000) proposing that visual WM is mainly represented in the right hemisphere (Baddeley, 2000), as well as evidence of a right hemisphere dominance for touch observation (e.g., Blakemore et al., 2005, Bolognini et al., 2013) and body representation (e.g., Longo et al., 2010, Tsakiris et al., 2007).

Two cortical areas of the right hemisphere were also targeted with TMS: the lateral occipital cortex (LOC) and dlPFC. In our starting hypothesis, the right LOC should act as a control site; this extrastriate area is implicated in the feature-based analysis of visual information (e.g., Amedi et al., 2001, Grill-Spector et al., 2001, Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001, Malach et al., 1995) but its role in the processing and the possible storing of body-related visual stimuli still needs to be determined (Gayet et al., 2018, Heuer et al., 2016, Pitcher et al., 2009, Xu, 2018, Xu and Chun, 2006). Conversely, the dlPFC was chosen according to a recent proposal that visual WM storage does not rely on sensory processing areas, but rather on specialized frontal (and parietal) areas not involved in low-level sensory processing per se (such as S1) (Xu, 2017). The right dlPFC is considered a key node of the WM network; indeed, recent studies suggest that its activation is related to the manipulation of stored information rather than to their maintenance (e.g., Barbey, Koenigs, & Grafman, 2013; C. Kim et al., 2015, Postle et al., 2006, Rowe et al., 2002, Veltman et al., 2003). However, previous TMS studies, where both the right and left dlPFC were stimulated, not always were successful in modulating participants’ performance during either visual and non-visual WM tasks (e.g., Bagherzadeh et al., 2016, Beynel et al., 2019, Chung et al., 2018, Hamidi et al., 2009). Hence, by stimulating LOC and dlPFC of the right hemisphere, we aimed at verifying the causal involvement of S1 in WM, assessing its main role in processing visual body-related stimuli (at variance with LOC) and in their storage (at variance with dlPFC); this would offer a better comprehension of its role in visual WM as compared to other areas of the same network. Indeed, by targeting two other functionally relevant areas, our study may provide stronger clues on the functional selectivity of S1 in visual WM tasks, rather than simply controlling for sensory and placebo rTMS confounds, as it would have happened if we had used a sham stimulation or a functionally ‘neutral’ area (such as the vertex) (Duecker & Sack, 2015).

In the second experiment (Experiment 2), we further explored the selective recruitment of the right S1 for body-related visual stimuli by assessing the effect of S1 rTMS on a delayed match-to-sample task depicting geometric shapes; we expected no effect of rTMS over S1 if visual stimuli are not body-related (e.g., Galvez-Pol, Calvo-Merino, et al., 2018).

Section snippets

Participants

Forty healthy male and female volunteers were initially recruited, 20 for each experiment. Two participants in Experiment 1 and one in Experiment 2 were then excluded because their averaged accuracy in the experimental tasks without rTMS was below chance level (<50%), while one participant of Experiment 2 dropped out before having completed all the experimental sessions, leaving thus the final analyzed sample to 18 participants in each experiment (Experiment 1: 4 males, mean age ± standard

Experiment 1

With respect to perceptual sensitivity (d’), results showed a main effect of factor Session (F3,51 = 5.74, p = .009, η2p = .251) but neither of factor Hemifield (F3,51 = 0.002, p = .96, η2p < .001) nor their interaction (F3,51 = 0.89, p = .451, η2p = .05), suggesting a modulation of participants’ performance independently of the stimulated hemifield. Hence, statistical significance of factor Session was further explored with post-hoc comparisons which highlighted that the stimulation of S1

Discussion

The present study has investigated whether S1 is causally involved in the maintenance of body-related information (i.e., hand gestures) in visual WM. We have found that participants’ performance in a delayed match-to-sample task visually presenting hand gestures to-be-remembered is enhanced by high-frequency rTMS delivered over the right S1. Importantly, this enhancement effect is area- and stimulus-specific: indeed, when rTMS is delivered over the right LOC or the right dlPFC (Experiment 1),

Data statement

Dataset, analysis, and stimuli are publicly achieved at the Open Science Framework (OSF): https://osf.io/k2eb9/.

Further information will be available from the corresponding author on a reasonable request.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Giacomo Guidali: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing - original draft. Camilla Roncoroni: Methodology, Investigation, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing - review & editing. Costanza Papagno: Methodology, Writing - review & editing. Nadia Bolognini: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing - original draft.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank Rolando Bonandrini for his valuable technical help with neuronavigation procedures.

References (124)

  • R. Cohen Kadosh et al.

    Virtual Dyscalculia Induced by Parietal-Lobe TMS Impairs Automatic Magnitude Processing

    Current Biology

    (2007)
  • T. Collins et al.

    TMS over posterior parietal cortex disrupts trans-saccadic visual stability

    Brain Stimulation

    (2018)
  • T.A. Daniel et al.

    Delayed match-to-sample in working memory: A BrainMap meta-analysis

    Biological Psychology

    (2016)
  • B. Forster et al.

    The attentive homunculus: ERP evidence for somatotopic allocation of attention in tactile search

    Neuropsychologia

    (2016)
  • J.J. Foster et al.

    Alpha-Band Activity Reveals Spontaneous Representations of Spatial Position in Visual Working Memory

    Current Biology

    (2017)
  • A. Galvez-Pol et al.

    Persistent recruitment of somatosensory cortex during active maintenance of hand images in working memory

    NeuroImage

    (2018)
  • A. Galvez-Pol et al.

    Revealing the body in the brain: An ERP method to examine sensorimotor activity during visual perception of body-related information

    Cortex

    (2020)
  • A. Galvez-Pol et al.

    Modulation of motor cortex activity in a visual working memory task of hand images

    Neuropsychologia

    (2018)
  • A. Galvez-Pol et al.

    Beyond action observation: Neurobehavioral mechanisms of memory for visually perceived bodies and actions

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2020)
  • S. Gayet et al.

    Visual Working Memory Storage Recruits Sensory Processing Areas

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2018)
  • B.K. Gerton et al.

    Shared and distinct neurophysiological components of the digits forward and backward tasks as revealed by functional neuroimaging

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • K. Grill-Spector et al.

    The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition

    Vision Research

    (2001)
  • G. Guidali et al.

    Keeping order in the brain: The supramarginal gyrus and serial order in short-term memory

    Cortex

    (2019)
  • M. Hamidi et al.

    Evaluating the role of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • Y. Hoshi et al.

    Visuospatial imagery is a fruitful strategy for the digit span backward task: A study with near-infrared optical tomography

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2000)
  • C. Keysers et al.

    A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • C. Kim et al.

    The role of the frontopolar cortex in manipulation of integrated information in working memory

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2015)
  • Y. Ku et al.

    Sequential roles of primary somatosensory cortex and posterior parietal cortex in tactile-visual cross-modal working memory: A single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (spTMS) study

    Brain Stimulation

    (2015)
  • D.R. Lametti et al.

    Cognitive Neuroscience: The Neural Basis of Motor Learning by Observing

    Current Biology

    (2016)
  • S. Leiberg et al.

    Effects of memory load on cortical oscillatory activity during auditory pattern working memory

    Brain Research

    (2006)
  • M.R. Longo et al.

    More than skin deep: Body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • B. Luber et al.

    Enhancement of human cognitive performance using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

    NeuroImage

    (2014)
  • S.J. Luck et al.

    Visual working memory capacity: From psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2013)
  • R. Luria et al.

    The contralateral delay activity as a neural measure of visual working memory

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2016)
  • H.R. McGregor et al.

    Functional Plasticity in Somatosensory Cortex Supports Motor Learning by Observing

    Current Biology

    (2016)
  • P. Molenberghs et al.

    Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2012)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • M. Orth et al.

    The cortical silent period: Intrinsic variability and relation to the waveform of the transcranial magnetic stimulation pulse

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2004)
  • M.A. Parra et al.

    Neural correlates of shape-color binding in visual working memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2014)
  • A. Pisoni et al.

    Cortical dynamics underpinning the self-other distinction of touch : A TMS-EEG study

    Neuroimage

    (2018)
  • D. Pitcher et al.

    Triple Dissociation of Faces, Bodies, and Objects in Extrastriate Cortex

    Current Biology

    (2009)
  • S. Rossi et al.

    Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2009)
  • F. Roux et al.

    Working memory and neural oscillations: Alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information?

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2014)
  • M. Schaefer et al.

    Embodied empathy for tactile events: Interindividual differences and vicarious somatosensory responses during touch observation

    NeuroImage

    (2012)
  • A. Amedi et al.

    Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2001)
  • Arslanova, I., Galvez-Pol, A., Calvo-Merino, B., & Forster, B. (2019). Searching for bodies: ERP evidence for...
  • A. Baddeley

    Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2012)
  • Y. Bagherzadeh et al.

    Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances working memory

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2016)
  • I. Bakulin et al.

    Combining HF rTMS over the left DLPFC with concurrent cognitive activity for the offline modulation of working memory in healthy volunteers: A proof-of-concept study

    Brain Sciences

    (2020)
  • M. Bauer et al.

    Tactile spatial attention enhances gamma-band activity in somatosensory cortex and reduces low-frequency activity in parieto-occipital areas

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • Cited by (2)

    View full text