Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 25 September 2020
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.565772

The Effectiveness of Online Messages for Promoting Smoking Cessation Resources: Predicting Nationwide Campaign Effects From Neural Responses in the EX Campaign

Ralf Schmälzle1† Nicole Cooper2,3† Matthew Brook O’Donnell2 Steven Tompson3,4 Sangil Lee5 Jennifer Cantrell6 Jean M. Vettel3,4,7 Emily B. Falk2,5,8*
  • 1Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts and Sciences, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States
  • 2Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 3U.S. Army Research Laboratory, Aberdeen Proving Ground, Adelphi, MD, United States
  • 4Department of Bioengineering, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 6New York University School of Global Public Health, New York, NY, United States
  • 7Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States
  • 8Wharton Marketing Department, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States

What are the key ingredients that make some persuasive messages resonate with audiences and elicit action, while others fail? Billions of dollars per year are put towards changing human behavior, but it is difficult to know which messages will be the most persuasive in the field. By combining novel neuroimaging techniques and large-scale online data, we examine the role of key health communication variables relevant to motivating action at scale. We exposed a sample of smokers to anti-smoking web-banner messages from a real-world campaign while measuring message-evoked brain response patterns via fMRI, and we also obtained subjective evaluations of each banner. Neural indices were derived based on: (i) message-evoked activity in specific brain regions; and (ii) spatially distributed response patterns, both selected based on prior research and theoretical considerations. Next, we connected the neural and subjective data with an independent, objective outcome of message success, which is the per-banner click-through rate in the real-world campaign. Results show that messages evoking brain responses more similar to signatures of negative emotion and vividness had lower online click-through-rates. This strategy helps to connect and integrate the rapidly growing body of knowledge about brain function with formative research and outcome evaluation of health campaigns, and could ultimately further disease prevention efforts.

Introduction

Mass media health campaigns are a key component of public health promotion (Wakefield et al., 2010), and the internet has become one of the most prominent channels for dissemination of health campaign messages (Shi et al., 2018). The need to maximize the effectiveness of health messaging is clear given the enormous burden associated with preventable diseases (Mokdad et al., 2004) and the need for efficiency in choosing messages. In the case of tobacco, for example, prevention efforts have only about 5% of the budget available to tobacco companies (Farrelly et al., 2003; CDC/CDCTobaccoFree, 2019). Despite important progress (Cho, 2011; Kim and Cappella, 2019; Sutton et al., 2019), the creation and selection of campaign messages remain a mix of art and science (Noar, 2011; Rice and Atkin, 2012). A deeper understanding of the factors associated with message success is critical for designing more efficient and effective health campaigns.

Neuroimaging is one means of capturing message-evoked responses and improving prediction of health message effectiveness (Falk, 2010; Falk and Scholz, 2018; Schmälzle et al., 2017). A growing body of neuroimaging work has demonstrated that message-evoked brain responses can predict key communication outcomes, such as changes in individuals’ health behavior (Chua et al., 2011; Falk et al., 2011, 2015a; Wang et al., 2015; Riddle et al., 2016; Vezich et al., 2016; Cooper et al., 2017; Zelle et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2018), as well as large-scale outcomes like calls to smoking quitlines (Falk et al., 2015b, 2016). Methodologically, the most widely used approach to capture message-evoked brain responses has relied on assessing the average signal in specific brain regions (Falk and Scholz, 2018). These approaches have primarily focused on the role of regions broadly implicated in positive valuation and personal relevance, such as the nucleus accumbens (NAcc; Knutson et al., 2014; Genevsky and Knutson, 2015; Genevsky et al., 2017; Scholz et al., 2017) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC; Falk et al., 2012; Venkatraman et al., 2015; Scholz et al., 2017, 2019; Doré et al., 2019a).

Novel neuroimaging methods suggest that using the spatial pattern of activation across the whole brain can carry important information beyond univariate (regional) activity and can improve the prediction of important outcomes. For example, brain activity “signature maps” incorporate spatial information about patterns of activity (or signatures) throughout the whole brain, and can accurately predict responses such as the strength of negative emotion evoked by effective images as in the Picture induced negative emotion signature (PINES, Chang et al., 2015), or craving in the context of food choice (Cosme et al., 2020), as well as message-relevant outcomes (Kaplan et al., 2015; Doré et al., 2019b). Comparable signature maps have also been trained for several other domains (Wager et al., 2013; Eisenbarth et al., 2016), including positive emotion and vividness during future-oriented scenario thinking (Lee et al., 2020), among other domains. These signatures have the potential for wide application; that is, they could aid the selection of messages that can elicit brain activity captured by these signature maps.

Here, we test the utility of multivariate brain activity signature maps related to selected key theoretical constructs in predicting health campaign outcomes. Specifically, we focus on neural signatures of negative emotion, positive emotion, and message vividness. Within the health communication literature, negative emotions, such as fear, have been studied extensively (Rogers, 1975; Witte and Allen, 2000; Tannenbaum et al., 2015). For example, highlighting the risks of continuing a behavior (like smoking) can motivate change (Rogers, 1975; Witte, 1992; Renner and Schwarzer, 2003). On the other hand, inducing negative emotions about the process of behavior change (e.g., highlighting the difficulty of change) can also discourage change (Witte, 1994; Maloney et al., 2011; Ruiter et al., 2014).

A second concept, particularly prominent in the social marketing literature (Lee and Kotler, 2011), is positive emotion. Positive emotions are related to the activation of the appetitive motivational system (Elliot, 2013; Lang and Bradley, 2013), and messages that convey the positive value of health behavior change should promote action (Myrick, 2015; Nabi, 2015; Guan and Monahan, 2017), as shown by the success of commercial advertising (Tellis, 2003; Siegel, 2013).

An additional key element of messaging relates to message vividness (Zillmann, 2006; Spence et al., 2017; Blondé and Girandola, 2018; Ophir et al., 2019). On the one hand, highly vivid messages can boost the message’s target emotion and render the described situation more plausible and concrete (D’Argembeau et al., 2008; Ji et al., 2016). For example, in the context of an anti-smoking campaign, vividly imagining the negative consequences of smoking could encourage quitting. However, vivid imagining could also backfire if a message prompted the targeted viewer to vividly imagine how difficult it will be to enact the desired behavior, and instead induce avoidance of behavior change. For example, vividly imagining how hard it will be to quit smoking could reduce the chances that a smoker attempts to quit. In sum, although there is an agreement that emotion and vividness play a role in health message design, they may help or hinder message success.

Using the “Ex” tobacco cessation campaign as a real-world test case, we examine whether neuroimaging methods focused on multivariate signatures of key health communication constructs are related to key campaign outcomes. We suggest an approach that helps integrate the health communication literature on emotion and vividness with novel neuroimaging methods. To better understand the processes that predict the success of online messaging, we collected neural data while smokers were exposed to online banner ads promoting smoking cessation. We computed both regional (univariate) and pattern-based (signature map) indices for each banner across this neural sample based on the theoretical considerations outlined above. We used an online sample of smokers exposed to the same banner ads to obtain subjective evaluations about theorized constructs (i.e., negative and positive emotional responses and vividness), to gain insight into whether multivariate brain indices concur with subjective evaluations. We find evidence, from neural and subjective data, that in the context of this campaign, negative emotion and vividness were negatively related to online ad success. Further, we find that although neural and survey data are positively related, prediction of online ad success is improved when using both sources of information, suggesting that theoretically-motivated neural signature maps could ultimately aid in the design and selection of effective messaging in conjunction with more traditional survey-based methods.

Materials and Methods

This study brings together data from three separate sources: (1) Online banner messages and corresponding click-through-rates from the Truth Initiative’s Ex smoking cessation campaign; (2) neural data recorded while a group of smokers was exposed to the Ex banners; and (3) subjective evaluations of the Ex banners obtained from an online test audience of smokers. Below, we provide methodological details on each data source.

The Truth Initiative’s Ex Campaign

Banner Messages

Twenty-three banner messages, or ads, aired online as part of the Ex campaign, run by the Truth Initiative (formerly known as the American Legacy Foundation). The Ex campaign launched nationally on March 31, 2008, and continues to run digitally1. The banners were professionally produced and shared common design elements. All messages are characterized by an empathetic, smoker-to-smoker tone, with emphasis placed on disassociating smoking from common activities that may function as smoking triggers, such as drinking coffee, drinking alcohol, driving, or stressful situations (see Supplementary Figure S1 for examples). The animated banner messages contained text that empathized with smokers’ needs when quitting or struggles to overcome a specific smoking trigger, and some presented cartoon animations illustrating trigger situations. None contained sound, but some text was present on each. Each banner message ended promoting the EX campaign’s resources for smokers with a variant of the slogan “Re-learn life without cigarettes at BecomeanEx.org”, which viewers could click on to be brought to the campaign website for further information and quit smoking resources. The ads averaged 17.7 s in duration (range 13.9–30 s, SD = 3.9 s).

Population-Level Click-Through Rates (CTR)

The 23 banner messages were displayed nationally on websites appealing to the target audience of adult smokers. Viewing metrics were collected by the Truth Initiative, and the response rates described here were collected between January and August 2012. In this period, the banners had an average of 5,895,553 viewings (135,597,715 total across the 23 banners) and received an average of 9,936 clicks (228,517 total across the 23 banners). We computed the CTR for each banner as the number of viewers who clicked on the banner divided by the total viewers exposed to that banner. The average CTR aggregating across banners was 0.21%, and the variability of CTR across banners was quite large, ranging from 0.08–0.57%. One banner message received many fewer impressions than the other banners (9,545 impressions, compared to a range of 1.1–12.6 million impressions in all other banners); this banner was excluded from analysis due to this discrepancy. The CTR scores for individual banners were used to rank their effectiveness within the campaign.

Brain Responses: Neuroimaging Sample

Participants

Fifty smokers participated in the fMRI component of the study. Participants were recruited from the general population using Craigslist and UMClinicalStudies. Interested participants completed an eligibility screening phone call. To participate in the study, participants had to report smoking at least five cigarettes per day for the past month, have been a smoker for at least 12 months, be between the ages of 18 and 65, and meet standard fMRI eligibility criteria. All participants provided informed consent following the procedures of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Michigan. Three participants were excluded due to neurological abnormalities and technical issues. Five participants were excluded due to excessive head motion, at a threshold of greater than 3.5 mm displacement. This resulted in a final sample of 42 participants (24 males, 18 females, mean age = 32 years, SD = 13 years, range 19–64 years). Participants smoked an average of 13 (SD = 7) cigarettes per day.

Procedure

Participants watched and rated 23 animated banner messages while undergoing fMRI. Banners were presented in random order across participants. Immediately following each ad, participants were presented with a response screen with the statement “This makes me want to quit” and a five point rating scale (1 = definitely does not, 2 = does not, 3 = neutral, 4 = does, 5 = definitely does). They were allowed 4 s on the response screen, which was followed by fixation with a jittered ITI (mean = 4.1 s, range 3.1–7.5 s, SD = 1.1).

MRI Image Acquisition and Preprocessing

Neuroimaging data were acquired using a three Tesla GE Signa MRI scanner. One functional run of the banner messages task (304 volumes total) was acquired at the end of the scan session for each participant, preceded by other tasks that are not the focus of the current investigation. Functional images were recorded using a reverse spiral sequence (TR = 2,000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, 43 axial slices, FOV = 220 mm, slice thickness = 3 mm; voxel size = 3.44 × 3.44 × 3.0 mm). We also acquired in-plane T1-weighted images (43 slices; slice thickness = 3 mm; voxel size = 0.86 × 0.86 × 3.0 mm) and high-resolution T1-weighted images (SPGR; 124 slices; slice thickness = 1.02 × 1.02 × 1.2 mm) for use in coregistration and normalization.

Functional data were pre-processed and analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, Welcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK), AFNI, and Python. We utilized analysis and visualization tools from the NiBabel, Nilearn, and Seaborn packages (Abraham et al., 2014; Waskom et al., 2014). To allow for the stabilization of the BOLD signal, the first five volumes (10 s) of each run were discarded before analysis. Functional images were despiked using the 3d Despike program as implemented in the AFNI toolbox, corrected for differences in the time of slice acquisition using sinc interpolation (the first slice served as the reference slice), and spatially realigned to the first functional image. We then co-registered the functional and structural images using a two-stage procedure. First, in-plane T1 images were registered to the mean functional image. Next, high-resolution T1 images were registered to the in-plane image. After coregistration, high-resolution structural images were skull-stripped using the VBM8 toolbox for SPM2 and then normalized to the skull-stripped MNI template provided by FSL. Finally, functional images were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel (8 mm FWHM) for the activation analysis only (pattern-based analyses were computed using the same pipeline except for the smoothing step). The fMRI data were modeled for each participant using fixed-effects models within the general linear model as implemented in SPM8. The six rigid-body translation and rotation parameters derived from spatial realignment were also included as nuisance regressors in the first level models. Data were high-pass filtered with a cutoff of 128 s.

Item-Wise Modeling of Responses Towards Individual Banners

To assess the neural response to each ad (in service of computing a per-item response to each ad across the sample; see analysis strategy below), we created an item-wise first-level model in which each banner was modeled in a task regressor separate from a regressor representing all other banners (Mumford et al., 2014). The response periods were modeled in a single boxcar regressor, and fixation rest-periods constituted an implicit baseline. Neural responses to each banner were extracted by iterating over this procedure for all banners for each participant, and then averaging across all 42 participants to obtain a composite beta-map for each banner that served as the basis for extracting univariate or multivariate metrics. See Supplementary Figure S2 for group-averaged neural responses to the banner messages.

Region of Interest Analysis

We examined the neural response to each banner in selected brain regions of interest. Specifically, we extracted the average regional activity from the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the ventral striatum based on meta-analyses of the valuation system (Bartra et al., 2013). Following Knutson et al. (2014), we computed indices for positive and negative emotional arousal based on formulae that combine the regional activity of the anterior insula, the ventral striatum, and the anterior cingulate cortex. The region of interest values for each banner was rank-ordered for analyses below.

Multivariate Similarity Analysis

Having derived univariate metrics for each banner, we proceeded to multivariate analyses, which focus on whole-brain spatial patterns of banner-evoked brain activity. The PINES (picture-induced negative emotion signature) map was obtained from Chang et al. (2015), and the positive emotion and vividness maps from Lee et al. (2020; see also Supplementary Materials). The brain map of average activation across participants for each banner was then compared to the signature maps of interest for negative emotion (PINES), positive emotion, and vividness, respectively, by assessing the spatial correlation of the banner-wise maps and the signature map. The resulting values for each banner were rank-ordered and thus provide a non-parametric measure of fit between the signature map and the neural response to each banner within our sample of viewers.

Subjective Evaluations: Online Sample

Participants

We recruited 50 smokers using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk based on criteria that matched the target audience of the campaign and the fMRI sample (i.e., smoking at least five cigarettes per day for the past month, have been a smoker for at least 12 months, be between the ages of 18 and 65). The study was conducted following the procedures of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania. The final sample consisted of 44 raters after the exclusion of six participants who failed on an attention check (22 males, 22 females, mean age = 37 years, SD = 9 years, range 23–57 years). Participants smoked an average of 15 (SD = 6) cigarettes per day.

Subjective Evaluation Procedure

As in the fMRI study, each participant viewed each banner message. After viewing each message, participants were asked to evaluate the extent to which the ad generated negative and positive emotion (“This ad made me feel negative” and “This ad made me feel positive”); how vividly the ad made them imagine scenarios (“This ad made me vividly imagine reasons not to quit smoking” and “This ad made me vividly imagine reasons to quit smoking”; see Supplementary Materials for results related to the latter question item); and the ad’s effect on their motivation to quit smoking (“This makes me want to quit”). All responses were made on a five point scale from “strongly disagree”—“strongly agree.” Responses were averaged across participants on each dimension for each banner ad. These averages were then ranked within each dimension.

Analysis

We examined the relationships between online banner ad success (click-through rates—CTRs), the neural responses to each banner ad within a test audience, and subjective evaluations of each banner obtained from an online test audience. Thus, the unit of analysis is the individual banner ads from the EX campaign and the brain and subjective responses are collapsed across individuals in each sample to derive a group-level metric for each banner. See Figure 1 for a schematic overview of the analysis approach.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A set of 23 web-banners from the Legacy EX campaign served as stimuli. When these banners aired online, their page impressions and generated clicks were tracked, providing a click-through rate metric for each banner. In the fMRI study, the same banners were shown to a sample of 42 smokers and the pattern of brain activity evoked by each banner message was compared to theoretically-motivated neural signature maps from the fMRI literature. In a parallel online study, we obtained subjective evaluations of banner-evoked negative emotion, positive emotion, and vividness from a different group of 44 smokers. We then tested whether the brain-derived measures and subjective reports for each banner ad predict online click-through rates.

We first test the predictive capacity of univariate regions of interest used in previous work predicting message effectiveness, followed by tests of the multivariate maps of negative emotion, positive emotion, and vividness. Next, we test whether subjective evaluations, both from the online sample and the scanned participants, predict click through rates. Finally, we examine neural metrics and subjective evaluations in combined regression models to test whether having both sources of information improves the prediction of online banner message success (CTRs). In all correlation tests and regression models, we use the ranked measures described in each section above (results with unranked measures are in Supplementary Material). Ranks provide a non-parametric and robust way to compare variables across varied datasets. The extension of multiple regression on ranks has been studied by Iman and Conover (1979) and a similar approach has been used in previous work (Weber et al., 2015; Falk et al., 2016). In addition to the benefits of non-parametric analysis, ranked analyses are also desirable for practitioners who strive to select the best messages.

Results

Brain Measures Predict Online Banner Ad Success

To test the importance of three specific constructs3—negative emotion, positive emotion, and vividness—in the online success of the anti-smoking banner ads used in this campaign, we examined the average brain responses to the banners within key brain regions involved in these processes as well as recently developed multivariate patterns and related them to banner click-through-rates (CTR).

We began by testing for relationships between CTR and the per-banner neural indices derived from average brain activity in regions associated with the relevant constructs (negative and positive emotion) and in regions associated with message success in prior work. We found that per-banner brain activity in regions associated with negative emotional arousal (Knutson et al., 2014) was negatively related to CTR (r = −0.44, p = 0.04). In other words, banners that prompted the strongest response in these regions were those that received the least click-throughs. No significant relations were observed for the positive emotional arousal index (r = −0.01, p = 0.96) or for regions associated with positive valuation (VS: r = −0.14, p = 0.52; vmPFC: r = −0.05, p = 0.83).

Next, we computed the degree of similarity between the average whole-brain activation map for each banner and the brain signature maps for each of the three theoretically-motivated health communication constructs. The brain signature maps have been previously validated for their respective purpose (i.e., to predict negative or positive emotion and vividness, respectively) in independent datasets. We find that banners with higher similarity to the Picture-Induced-Negative-Emotion-Signature (PINES) have lower CTRs (r = −0.66, p = 0.0008, Figure 2, left)4. In other words, the more a banner evoked a brain response resembling the pattern of the negative emotion signature, the fewer clicks it generated in the real-world online campaign. We found that similarity to the positive emotion signature was marginally positive, though not significantly, associated with CTRs (r = 0.38, p = 0.082, Figure 2, middle). Banners with higher similarity to the vividness signature generated lower CTRs in the real-world campaign (r = −0.53, p = 0.011, Figure 2, right). That is, when the evoked brain response was more similar to the vivid brain signature, viewers were less likely to click through to online resources to quit smoking. These results suggest that banners that more closely resemble the brain maps for negative emotion and vividness were less successful online.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The degree to which the pattern of brain activity for each banner matches neural signature maps for negative and positive emotion, as well as vividness, is negatively linked to the online success (click-through-rates) of the same banners. Banners that prompt brain activity patterns that better match negative emotion and vividness signatures tend to have lower online success (i.e., lower click-through rates).

Subjective Evaluations Predict Online Banner Ad Success

Next, we examined the subjective evaluations collected from our online sample of MTurk smokers. Participants rated the extent to which each ad generated negative or positive emotion, and how vividly the ad made them imagine reasons not to quit smoking. There was a negative relationship between negative emotion ratings and CTR (r = −0.58, p = 0.005; Figure 3, left), such that banners eliciting higher levels of negative emotion had lower CTRs. Positive emotion ratings were positively, but only marginally associated with CTR (r = 0.39, p = 0.07; Figure 3, middle). Finally, vividness showed a strong negative relationship to CTR (r = −0.81; p = 0.000006; Figure 3, right), such that banners eliciting higher levels of vividness had lower CTRs. These results parallel the findings described above for the multivariate signature maps of emotion and vividness.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Linking subjective reports of negative emotion, positive emotion, and vividness per banner to the population-level click-through rate generated by the same banners.

Brain Measures Improve Predictions of Online Banner Ad Success

Next, we tested whether the neural metrics provide additional explanatory power when combined in multiple regression models with subjective evaluations. To do this, we compared the R2 values for models with subjective evaluations alone to R2 values for models with brain similarity metrics combined with subjective evaluations.

We first consider negative emotion. The addition of the brain similarity metric to a model containing subjective evaluations alone increased the explained variance in CTR from R2 = 0.33 to R2 = 0.45, which is a marginal improvement in model fit (F(1,19) = 4.1, p = 0.057; see Supplementary Figure S3 for correlations between all brain and subjective metrics). We find a similar result for vividness. The addition of the brain similarity metric to a model containing subjective evaluations increased the explained variance in CTR from R2 = 0.65 to R2 = 0.73, which is a significant improvement in model fit (F(1,19) = 5.71, p = 0.027)5. The addition of the positive emotion brain similarity metric to a model containing subjective evaluations of positive emotion did not significantly increase the explained variance in CTR (R2 = 0.16 to R2 = 0.23; model comparison F(1,19) = 1.87, p = 0.19), though the effect size was comparable to the other construct maps.

Thus, although the brain similarity metrics and subjective evaluations are correlated with each other (Supplementary Figure S3), they are not fully redundant. Practically, having information from both sources is useful for prediction, as the combined models are a significantly better fit than models with subjective evaluations alone.

Discussion

We recorded neural responses and obtained subjective evaluations from two samples of viewers exposed to anti-smoking banner messages from the Truth Initiative’s online EX campaign, and we linked these data with large-scale metrics of effectiveness for the same banners at scale. We find that multivariate brain maps assessing negative emotion and vividness in a small group of smokers who underwent brain imaging were strongly and negatively associated with the success of the same banner messages online, with over 135 million viewings during the nationwide online campaign. Further, we find that these multivariate brain metrics improve predictions of message success relative to regional activation in select brain areas of interest, and relative to exclusively using self-reported evaluations of the ads made by a separate sample of smokers. These data support the utility of neuroimaging techniques in understanding and forecasting public health campaign effectiveness (Falk and Scholz, 2018).

Although the EX campaign was highly successful overall (Vallone et al., 2010, 2011), there was variability in the online success of individual banner messages. Specifically, our results show that banner messages which elicited a brain response more similar to a negative emotion signature (Chang et al., 2015) were less successful in attracting clicks from viewers in this context. These results are in line with the notion that increased negative emotion can detract from the effectiveness of messages (Peters et al., 2013; Ruiter et al., 2014), and recent findings showing that although some kinds of emotion (e.g., fear; Tannenbaum et al., 2015) can improve campaign outcomes, strong negative emotion in smoking cessation messages does not always increase the call-volume to quitlines (Farrelly et al., 2011). As such, people may be less likely to take action towards quitting when messages prompt negative emotion or are framed in ways that highlight the problems with past behavior (Rothman and Salovey, 1997). This could be particularly true in contexts where viewers are likely to react defensively, such as smokers viewing anti-smoking messages (Sweeney and Moyer, 2015; Memish et al., 2017) or when vividly imagining how difficult it might be to quit. Furthermore, particularly in the new media environment, banner messages are presented alongside other information that they compete with for the viewer’s limited attentional resources, and the self-paced and easily terminated nature of browsing behavior might promote avoidance of information that elicits negative emotion.

We also examined the role of vividness in message effectiveness and found that brain responses more consistent with high levels of vividness were associated with lower population level click through rates. As is the case for negative emotion, prior research has shown competing effects of vividness (Taylor and Thompson, 1982; Collins et al., 1988; Smith and Shaffer, 2000). For example, it seems intuitive that higher message vividness—often in tandem with negative emotion—could improve message effectiveness, for example, if people vividly imagine the consequences of their behaviors. However, other reports find that high vividness can be detrimental if creative special effects overshadow the message’s main point (Smith and Shaffer, 2000; Guadagno et al., 2011). Another explanation is that vividly imagining how hard it will be to quit smoking may demotivate the approach behavior of clicking on the link to obtain more information about quitting. Indeed, the subjective data from MTurk raters support this idea, as the ratings of banner-elicited negative thoughts (“This ad made me vividly imagine reasons not to quit smoking”) were very strongly anti-correlated with message success (r = −0.81). Future work may test this possibility by, for example, using think-aloud protocols to tap into participants’ thought processes after message exposure (Cacioppo et al., 1997; Pei et al., 2019).

Finally, banner messages eliciting a brain response more similar to the positive emotion signature were marginally more successful in attracting clicks online (r = 0.38), but the relationship was statistically not significant. The same pattern emerged for the subjective evaluations, i.e., a marginally positive relationship between the per-banner ratings of positive emotion and click-through rate that was not statistically significant (r = 0.37). Of note, the brain-based and the subjective measures also correlated positively (r = 0.29), but again not significantly. Work in other contexts, such as predicting crowd-funding outcomes and video virality using brain data, has made a case for positive arousal as a key factor (Genevsky et al., 2017; Tong et al., 2020), but in the current dataset neither the univariate analyses, the multivariate signature, or the subjective evaluations correlated significantly with the click-through measures. One possible explanation is that the relationships would have been significant with a larger number of banner messages, which is a naturally limiting factor in campaigns that use only a small-to-moderate number of messages (here 23 banners). Alternatively, this could suggest that either the context of health prevention messaging or the specific outcome behavior of clicking on an online banner may be important to consider for such brain-behavior analyses.

Concerning the methods used in health prevention neuroimaging, we show that neural signatures developed to detect images-evoked negative emotion (Chang et al., 2015) and neural signatures based on data tracking vividness (Lee et al., 2020; also see Supplementary Materials) were indicative of negative responses to health messaging. This suggests that the brain activity signature maps can be useful outside of the context in which they were directly developed (i.e., maps of basic processes like negative emotion and vividness being successfully applied to predict the success of health messages). We also find that subjective evaluations of the emotions and vividness induced by the banner ads correlate positively with expression of the corresponding signature maps, providing support for our assumption that the signature maps correlate with the constructs for which they were developed—even in very different contexts. That said, although the relationships between subjective evaluations of the ads and multivariate brain metrics to click-through-rates are closely related, information from both sources is useful for the prediction of campaign outcomes. More broadly, this approach offers a principled strategy to integrate neural and subjective data sources.

This demonstration of the viability to use multivariate brain signatures to predict message success also suggests the possibility to train signatures for tapping into sub-aspects of message-induced persuasion. Specifically, while we chose the signatures based on theoretical considerations in the context of health communication, one could attempt to develop new signatures that are directly trained to tap into phenomena of interest rather than using available signatures (Chang et al., 2015; Cosme et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2020). The long-term goal of discovering such markers will be greatly aided by high-quality behavioral outcome metrics, such as large-scale click-through-rates. The click-through rates used to operationalize effectiveness in the current study are an objective behavioral measure of message success at the aggregate level, comparable to audience ratings or sales revenue in other domains, or the persuasion rate more broadly (DellaVigna and Gentzkow, 2010). In terms of the goal of the banners to influence people towards taking the first step towards quitting, i.e., to sign up for quit resources, the click-through rates measured here are a key outcome metric (Rhodes and Ewoldsen, 2013).

Importantly, population-level effects on click-through rates can only emerge if the relevant message—in this case, the banner—can collectively influence a large number of individuals (Imhof et al., 2017, 2020; Grall and Schmälzle, 2020). Our strategy to capture brain activity within a smaller sample of current smokers exposed to the same messages probes this collective-level response to messaging. Moreover, message-evoked brain activity patterns from a relatively small group of individuals can be linked to population-level behavior in response to the banner messages (Knutson and Genevsky, 2018). Going forward, social media-based health communication offers ideal opportunities to measure mass behavior at scale (O’Donnell and Falk, 2015; Tan et al., 2016; Matz et al., 2017) and to jointly examine mechanisms and effects of health prevention messaging.

With this in mind, future research should expand the range of methods used to capture responses to health prevention messages. For instance, Imhof et al. (2017, 2020) used fMRI and EEG methods to measure message-evoked brain responses towards the same set of health messages and demonstrated the possibility of EEG-informed fMRI analyses. Such a strategy seems feasible for the current design and implementing it in future studies promises several insights: if both methods can tap into the relevant message-evoked processes, they can make measurements more robust and provide cross-validation; if, on the other hand, the methods tap into different processes that are relevant for CTR or other outcomes, then it will be important to tease apart which methods are most sensitive for the specific process. The same reasoning also applies to other methods used to study biobehavioral responses to health messages, such as eye-tracking (e.g., Lochbühler et al., 2016), psychophysiological measures, or functional-near-infrared spectroscopy.

Another issue that awaits further research relates to the match between the samples used for message testing and the campaign target audience. The EX campaign was created to help current smokers who are open to quitting and accordingly we recruited only smokers for the fMRI and online studies. However, it will be interesting to examine which specific audience characteristics and individual differences—demographic, psychological, and behavioral—must be matched between the testing samples and the campaign target audience. For instance, it has been shown that brain responses to health risk communication differed between individuals depending on their preexisting level of risk perception (Schmälzle et al., 2013) and that responses to anti-drug messages differed between individuals based on their drug use risk (Huskey et al., 2017). Thus, while it is common wisdom in the social sciences that samples need to be representative of the population to warrant robust inferences, questions remain whether certain responses to messages are obligatory across all receivers and which individual differences must be taken into account. In the current study, the neural and subjective measures obtained for each banner were collapsed across individuals in the fMRI and online samples, respectively, to derive measurements for each banner and relate these to the per-banner CTRs. However, given that individual variability exists in how both the neural and the online samples respond to each message, another question will be how many individuals are needed to obtain robust group-level metrics. Kim and Cappella (2019) suggest that collapsing ratings from about 25 raters per message provides stable ratings when norming health messages for constructs like perceived effectiveness or perceived argument strength. Prior neuroimaging studies aiming to use average brain responses from small groups to predict larger campaign success have used sample sizes between 18 and 47 (Knutson and Genevsky, 2018). Our samples of over 40 participants are within this range, but more work is needed to gauge the individual differences and develop message-testing protocols for neuroimaging that establish how many brain responses are required for optimal prediction.

Summary and Conclusion

In sum, we bring together perspectives from the neuroscience of emotion and persuasion (Lindquist et al., 2012; Falk and Scholz, 2018) with large-scale public health campaigns (Wakefield et al., 2010; Rice and Atkin, 2012) by linking message-wise brain effects to population-level message effects. We find that multivariate brain signatures of negative emotion and vividness in response to anti-smoking messages in this context were negatively associated with the real-world success of the messages, i.e., the click-through rates generated by individual banner messages. This strategy yields insights into the neurocognitive processes of message reception and behavior change and may thus help to link the rapidly growing body of knowledge about brain function with formative and evaluation research of health campaigns (Noar, 2011; Rice and Atkin, 2012). Integrating functional neuroimaging with large-scale outcome data about campaign effects is a promising avenue for future work that can improve public communication campaigns as well as other forms of information dissemination.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

RS, NC, MO’D and EF formulated the investigation. RS and NC performed the analysis in discussion with JV and EF. SL generated the valence and vividness signature maps. RS, NC, JV and EF wrote the article. MO’D and ST collected the data and provided analysis tools. Truth Initiative: collected and provided data and analysis tools. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported in part by grants from The Michigan Center of Excellence in Cancer Communication Research/NIH-P50 CA101451 (PI: Strecher); NIH New Innovator Award/NIH 1DP2DA03515601 (PI: EF); U.S. Army Research Laboratory, including work under Cooperative Agreement Number W911NF-10-2-0022; the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (FA8650-17-C-7712 and 140D0419C0093); the NIH/National Cancer Institute; and FDA Center for Tobacco Products Grant P50CA179546 (pilot grant). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of any funding agencies.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge Richard Gonzalez, Sonya Dal Cin, Victor Strecher, and Lawrence An for collaboration on a larger study relevant to this work; Francis Tinney Jr., Kristin Shumaker, Li Chen, Nicolette Gregor, Becky Lau, Larissa Svintsitski, Cole Schaffer for assistance with data collection; Bruce Doré for discussion; and Donna Vallone, Jeffrey Costantino, Amanda Richardson, Aaron Mushro and the Truth Initiative for provision of study stimuli.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.becomeanex.org
  2. ^ http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm
  3. ^ Of note, the responses to the question “This ad makes me motivated to quit smoking” did not significantly predict population-level click-through-rates—neither in the fMRI sample (r = −0.01, n.s.), nor in the online sample (r = −0.09, n.s.).
  4. ^ To test whether the negative arousal univariate index and the negative emotion multivariate signature map capture the same variance in CTR, we entered both neural metrics into a regression model predicting CTR. When these predictors are combined in the same model, the negative emotion brain predictor is significantly related to CTR (t(19) = −3.71, p = 0.001), and negative arousal is not (t(19) = −1.95, p = 0.067), suggesting that similarity to the multivariate map provides unique information.
  5. ^ Subjective evaluations did not significantly improve model fit beyond what was explained by the neural signature of negative emotion (R = 0.44 to R = 0.45; model comparison F(1,19) = 0.42, p = 0.52) or positive emotion (R = 0.14 to R = 0.23; model comparison F(1,19) = 2.18, p = 0.16). A combined model with subjective evaluations of both subjective and neural signatures of vividness did improve model fit beyond a model with neural signatures alone (R = 0.29 to R = 0.73; model comparison F(1,19) = 31.2, p = 2.2e-05).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2020.565772/full#supplementary-material.

References

Abraham, A., Pedregosa, F., Eickenberg, M., Gervais, P., Mueller, A., Kossaifi, J., et al. (2014). Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn. Front. Neuroinform. 8:14. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T., and Kable, J. W. (2013). The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage 76, 412–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blondé, J., and Girandola, F. (2018). Are vivid (vs. pallid) threats persuasive? Examining the effects of threat vividness in health communications. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 40, 36–48. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2017.1412969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., von Hippel, W., and Ernst, J. M. (1997). Mapping cognitive structures and processes through verbal content: the thought-listing technique. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 65, 928–940. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.6.928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CDC/CDCTobaccoFree. (2019). Economic Trends in Tobacco. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Available online at: https://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/fact_sheets/economics/econ_facts/.

Chang, L. J., Gianaros, P. J., Manuck, S. B., Krishnan, A., and Wager, T. D. (2015). A sensitive and specific neural signature for picture-induced negative affect. PLoS Biol. 13:e1002180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, H. (2011). Health Communication Message Design: Theory and Practice. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Chua, H. F., Ho, S. S., Jasinska, A. J., Polk, T. A., Welsh, R. C., Liberzon, I., et al. (2011). Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 426–427. doi: 10.1038/nn.2761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., Wood, J. V., and Thompson, S. C. (1988). The vividness effect: elusive or illusory? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 24, 1–18. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(88)90041-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, N., Bassett, D. S., and Falk, E. B. (2017). Coherent activity between brain regions that code for value is linked to the malleability of human behavior. Sci. Rep. 7:43250. doi: 10.1038/srep43250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosme, D., Zeithamova, S., Stice, E., and Berkman, E. T. (2020). Multivariate neural signatures for health neuroscience: assessing spontaneous regulation during food choice. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa002 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Argembeau, A., Xue, G., Lu, Z.-L., Van der Linden, M., and Bechara, A. (2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future. NeuroImage 40, 398–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DellaVigna, S., and Gentzkow, M. (2010). Persuasion: empirical evidence. Annu. Rev. Econom. 2, 643–669. doi: 10.1146/annurev.economics.102308.124309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doré, B. P., Cooper, N., Scholz, C., O’Donnell, M. B., and Falk, E. B. (2019a). Cognitive regulation of ventromedial prefrontal activity evokes lasting change in the perceived self-relevance of persuasive messaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 2571–2580. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doré, B. P., Tompson, S. H., O’Donnell, M. B., An, L. C., Strecher, V., and Falk, E. B. (2019b). Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation moderate the predictive value of affective and value-related brain responses to persuasive messages. J. Neurosci. 39, 1293–1300. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1651-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenbarth, H., Chang, L. J., and Wager, T. D. (2016). Multivariate brain prediction of heart rate and skin conductance responses to social threat. J. Neurosci. 36, 11987–11998. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3672-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J. (2013). Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., and Lieberman, M. D. (2012). From neural responses to population behavior: neural focus group predicts population-level media effects. Psychol. Sci. 23, 439–445. doi: 10.1177/0956797611434964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B. (2010). Communication neuroscience as a tool for health psychologists. Health Psychol. 29, 355–357. doi: 10.1037/a0020427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., Whalen, D., and Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Neural activity during health messaging predicts reductions in smoking above and beyond self-report. Health Psychol. 30, 177–185. doi: 10.1037/a0022259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., Cascio, C. N., and Coronel, J. C. (2015a). Neural prediction of communication-relevant outcomes. Commun. Methods Measures 9, 30–54. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2014.999750

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., O’Donnell, M. B., Cascio, C. N., Tinney, F., Kang, Y., Lieberman, M. D., et al. (2015b). Self-affirmation alters the brain’s response to health messages and subsequent behavior change. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, 1977–1982. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1500247112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., O’Donnell, M. B., Tompson, S., Gonzalez, R., Dal Cin, S., Strecher, V., et al. (2016). Functional brain imaging predicts public health campaign success. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 204–214. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., and Scholz, C. (2018). Persuasion, influence, and value: perspectives from communication and social neuroscience. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69, 329–356. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrelly, M. C., Davis, K. C., Nonnemaker, J. M., Kamyab, K., and Jackson, C. (2011). Promoting calls to a quitline: quantifying the influence of message theme, strong negative emotions and graphic images in television advertisements. Tob. Control 20, 279–284. doi: 10.1136/tc.2010.042234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrelly, M. C., Niederdeppe, J., and Yarsevich, J. (2003). Youth tobacco prevention mass media campaigns: past, present and future directions. Tob. Control 12, i35–i47. doi: 10.1136/tc.12.suppl_1.i35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genevsky, A., and Knutson, B. (2015). Neural affective mechanisms predict market-level microlending. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1411–1422. doi: 10.1177/0956797615588467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genevsky, A., Yoon, C., and Knutson, B. (2017). When brain beats behavior: neuroforecasting crowdfunding outcomes. J. Neurosci. 37, 8625–8634. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1633-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grall, C., and Schmälzle, R. (2020). The coupled brains of captivated audiences. how suspense in a movie modulates collective brain dynamics. J. Media Psychol. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000271 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guadagno, R. E., Rhoads, K. L., and Sagarin, B. J. (2011). Figural vividness and persuasion: capturing the “elusive” vividness effect. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 626–638. doi: 10.1177/0146167211399585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guan, M., and Monahan, J. L. (2017). “Positive affect related to health and risk messaging,” in Encyclopedia of Health and Risk Message Design and Processing, ed R. Parrott (Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.013.268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huskey, R., Mangus, J. M., Turner, B. O., and Weber, R. (2017). The persuasion network is modulated by drug-use risk and predicts anti-drug message effectiveness. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1902–1915. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iman, R. L., and Conover, W. J. (1979). The use of the rank transform in regression. Technometrics 21, 499–509. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1979.10489820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imhof, M. A., Schmälzle, R., Renner, B., and Schupp, H. T. (2017). How real-life health messages engage our brains: shared processing of effective anti-alcohol videos. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1188–1196. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imhof, M. A., Schmälzle, R., Renner, B., and Schupp, H. T. (2020). Strong health messages increase audience brain coupling. NeuroImage 216:116527. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ji, J. L., Heyes, S. B., MacLeod, C., and Holmes, E. A. (2016). Emotional mental imagery as simulation of reality: fear and beyond—a tribute to Peter Lang. Behav. Ther. 47, 702–719. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, Y., Cooper, N., Pandey, P., Scholz, C., O’Donnell, M. B., Lieberman, M. D., et al. (2018). Effects of self-transcendence on neural responses to persuasive messages and health behavior change. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 115, 9974–9979. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1805573115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, J. T., Man, K., and Greening, S. G. (2015). Multivariate cross-classification: applying machine learning techniques to characterize abstraction in neural representations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:151. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., and Cappella, J. N. (2019). Reliable, valid and efficient evaluation of media messages. J. Commun. Manag. 23, 179–197. doi: 10.1108/JCOM-12-2018-0132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., and Genevsky, A. (2018). Neuroforecasting aggregate choice. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 110–115. doi: 10.1177/0963721417737877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Katovich, K., and Suri, G. (2014). Inferring affect from fMRI data. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 422–428. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., and Bradley, M. M. (2013). Appetitive and defensive motivation: goal-directed or goal-determined? Emot. Rev. 5, 230–234. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, N. R., and Kotler, P. (2011). Social Marketing: Influencing Behaviors for Good. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Lee, S. L., Parthasarathi, T., and Kable, J. (2020). The dorsal and ventral default mode networks are dissociably modulated by the valence and vividness of imagined events. BioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.05.21.109728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., and Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav. Brain Sci. 35, 121–143. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x11000446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lochbühler, K., Tang, K. Z., Souprountchouk, V., Campetti, D., Cappella, J. N., Kozlowski, L. T., et al. (2016). Using eye-tracking to examine how embedding risk corrective statements improves cigarette risk beliefs: implications for tobacco regulatory policy. Drug Alcohol Depend. 164, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.04.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. K., Lapinski, M. K., and Witte, K. (2011). Fear appeals and persuasion: a review and update of the extended parallel process model. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Comp. 5, 206–219. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00341.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matz, S. C., Kosinski, M., Nave, G., and Stillwell, D. J. (2017). Psychological targeting as an effective approach to digital mass persuasion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 114, 12714–12719. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1710966114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memish, K. E., Schüz, N., Frandsen, M., Ferguson, S. G., and Schüz, B. (2017). Using self-affirmation to increase the effects of emotive health warnings on smoking: a randomized exploratory trial. Nicotine Tob. Res. 19, 1238–1242. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntw167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mokdad, A. H., Marks, J. S., Stroup, D. F., and Gerberding, J. L. (2004). Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. JAMA 291, 1238–1245. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.10.1238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, J. A., Davis, T., and Poldrack, R. A. (2014). The impact of study design on pattern estimation for single-trial multivariate pattern analysis. NeuroImage 103, 130–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myrick, J. G. (2015). The Role of Emotions in Preventative Health Communication. Lanham, MA: Lexington Books.

Google Scholar

Nabi, R. L. (2015). Emotional flow in persuasive health messages. Health Commun. 30, 114–124. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2014.974129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noar, S. M. (2011). An audience-channel-message-evaluation (ACME) framework for health communication campaigns. Health Promot. Pract. 13, 481–488. doi: 10.1177/1524839910386901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Donnell, M. B., and Falk, E. B. (2015). Big data under the microscope and brains in social sontext integrating methods from computational social science and neuroscience. Anna. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 659, 274–289. doi: 10.1177/0002716215569446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ophir, Y., Brennan, E., Maloney, E. K., and Cappella, J. N. (2019). The effects of graphic warning labels’ vividness on message engagement and intentions to quit smoking. Communic. Res. 46, 619–638. doi: 10.1177/0093650217700226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pei, R., Schmälzle, R., O’Donnell, M., Kranzler, E., and Falk, E. (2019). Adolescents’ neural responses to tobacco prevention messages and subsequent sharing engagement. Am. J. Prev. Med. 56, S40–S48. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C., and Kok, G. (2013). Threatening communication: a critical re-analysis and a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal theory. Health Psychol. Rev. 7, S8–S31. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2012.703527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renner, B., and Schwarzer, R. (2003). Social-cognitive factors in health behavior change. Blackwell Series in Health Psychology and Behavioral Medicine. Social Psychological Foundations of Health and Illness, eds J. Suls and K. A. Wallston (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing), 169–196.

Google Scholar

Rhodes, N., and Ewoldsen, D. R. (2013). “Outcomes of persuasion: behavioral, cognitive, and social,” in The SAGE Handbook of Persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice, eds J. P. Dillard and L. Shen (Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.), 53–69.

Google Scholar

Rice, R. E., and Atkin, C. K. (2012). Public Communication Campaigns. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Riddle, P. J. Jr., Newman-Norlund, R. D., Baer, J., and Thrasher, J. F. (2016). Neural response to pictorial health warning labels can predict smoking behavioral change. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1802–1811. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R. W. (1975). A Protection Motivation Theory of fear appeals and attitude change. J. Psychol. 91, 93–114. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1975.9915803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, A. J., and Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. Psychol. Bull. 121, 3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiter, R. A. C., Kessels, L. T. E., Peters, G.-J. Y., and Kok, G. (2014). Sixty years of fear appeal research: current state of the evidence. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 63–70. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmälzle, R., Häcker, F., Renner, B., Honey, C. J., and Schupp, H. T. (2013). Neural correlates of risk perception during real-life risk communication. J. Neurosci. 33, 10340–10347. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5323-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmälzle, R., Renner, B., and Schupp, H. T. (2017). Health risk perception and risk communication. Policy Insigh. Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 163–169. doi: 10.1177/2372732217720223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, C., Baek, E. C., O’Donnell, M. B., Kim, H. S., Cappella, J. N., and Falk, E. B. (2017). A neural model of valuation and information virality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 114, 2881–2886. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1615259114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, C., Jovanova, M., Baek, E. C., and Falk, E. B. (2019). Media content sharing as a value-based decision. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 31, 83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, J., Poorisat, T., and Salmon, C. T. (2018). The use of social networking sites (SNSs) in health communication campaigns: review and recommendations. Health Commun. 33, 49–56. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2016.1242035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, E. (2013). Predictive Analytics: The Power to Predict Who Will Click, Buy, Lie, Or Die. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., and Shaffer, D. R. (2000). Vividness can undermine or enhance message processing: the moderating role of vividness congruency. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 769–779. doi: 10.1177/0146167200269003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, P. R., Westerman, D., and Rice, R. G. (2017). “Exemplification theory in health and risk messaging,” in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication, ed. R. Parrott (New York, NY: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/acref/9780190455378.001.0001/

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, J. A., Yang, S., and Cappella, J. N. (2019). Perceived effectiveness of objective features of pictorial warning messages. Tob. Control 28, e24–e30. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2018-054488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeney, A. M., and Moyer, A. (2015). Self-affirmation and responses to health messages: a meta-analysis on intentions and behavior. Health Psychol. 34, 149–159. doi: 10.1037/hea0000110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, C., Niculae, V., Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., and Lee, L. (2016). “Winning arguments: interaction dynamics and persuasion strategies in good-faith online discussions,” in WWW ’16: Proceedings of the 25th International Conference on World Wide Web, Montréeal, QC, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Tannenbaum, M. B., Hepler, J., Zimmerman, R. S., Saul, L., Jacobs, S., Wilson, K., et al. (2015). Appealing to fear: a meta-Analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories. Psychol. Bull. 141, 1178–1204. doi: 10.1037/a0039729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E., and Thompson, S. C. (1982). Stalking the elusive “vividness” effect. Psychol. Rev. 89, 155–181. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.89.2.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tellis, G. J. (2003). Effective Advertising: Understanding When, How and Why Advertising works. London: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Tong, L. C., Acikalin, M. Y., Genevsky, A., Shiv, B., and Knutson, B. (2020). Brain activity forecasts video engagement in an internet attention market. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 117, 6936–6941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1905178117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallone, D. M., Duke, J. C., Cullen, J., McCausland, K. L., and Allen, J. A. (2011). Evaluation of EX: a national mass media smoking cessation campaign. Am. J. Public Health 101, 302–309. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2009.190454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallone, D. M., Duke, J. C., Mowery, P. D., McCausland, K. L., Xiao, H., Costantino, J. C., et al. (2010). The impact of EX: results from a pilot smoking-cessation media campaign. Am. J. Prev. Med. 38, S312–S318. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venkatraman, V., Dimoka, A., Pavlou, P. A., Vo, K., Hampton, W., Bollinger, B., et al. (2015). Predicting advertising success beyond traditional measures: new insights from neurophysiological methods and market response modeling. J. Mark. Res. 52, 436–452. doi: 10.1509/jmr.13.0593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vezich, S., Katzman, P. L., Ames, D. L., Falk, E. B., and Lieberman, M. D. (2016). Modulating the neural bases of persuasion: why/how, gain/loss, and users/non-users. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 12, 283–297. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Lindquist, M. A., Roy, M., Woo, C.-W., and Kross, E. (2013). An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N. Engl. J. Med. 368, 1388–1397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wakefield, M. A., Loken, B., and Hornik, R. C. (2010). Use of mass media campaigns to change health behaviour. Lancet 376, 1261–1271. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(10)60809-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, A.-L., Lowen, S. B., Romer, D., Giorno, M., and Langleben, D. D. (2015). Emotional reaction facilitates the brain and behavioural impact of graphic cigarette warning labels in smokers. Tob. Control 24, 225–232. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waskom, M., Botvinnik, O., Hobson, P., Cole, J. B., Halchenko, Y., Hoyer, S., et al. (2014). seaborn: v0.5.0 (November 2014). Available online at: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.12710.

Weber, R., Huskey, R., Mangus, J. M., Westcott-Baker, A., and Turner, B. O. (2015). Neural predictors of message effectiveness during counterarguing in antidrug campaigns. Commun. Monogr. 82, 4–30. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2014.971414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Commun. Monogr. 59, 329–349. doi: 10.1080/03637759209376276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, K. (1994). Fear control and danger control: a test of the extended parallel process model (EPPM). Commun. Monogr. 61, 113–134. doi: 10.1080/03637759409376328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, K., and Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ. Behav. 27, 591–615. doi: 10.1177/109019810002700506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelle, S. L., Gates, K. M., Fiez, J. A., Sayette, M. A., and Wilson, S. J. (2017). The first day is always the hardest: functional connectivity during cue exposure and the ability to resist smoking in the initial hours of a quit attempt. NeuroImage 151, 24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zillmann, D. (2006). Exemplification effects in the promotion of safety and health. J. Commun. 56, 221–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00291.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fMRI, health communication, smoking, banner ads, advertising, click-through rate

Citation: Schmälzle R, Cooper N, O’Donnell MB, Tompson S, Lee S, Cantrell J, Vettel JM and Falk EB (2020) The Effectiveness of Online Messages for Promoting Smoking Cessation Resources: Predicting Nationwide Campaign Effects From Neural Responses in the EX Campaign. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:565772. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.565772

Received: 26 May 2020; Accepted: 20 August 2020;
Published: 25 September 2020.

Edited by:

Teresa Liu-Ambrose, University of British Columbia, Canada

Reviewed by:

Ayşe Ikinci Keleş, Niğde Ömer Halisdemir University, Turkey
Omur Gulsum Deniz, Abant Izzet Baysal University, Turkey

Copyright © 2020 Schmälzle, Cooper, O’Donnell, Tompson, Lee, Cantrell, Vettel and Falk. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emily B. Falk, falk@asc.upenn.edu

These authors share first authorship

Download