Introduction

The elaborate division of labor of eusocial insects, such as ants, bees, wasps, and termites, is one important factor responsible for their ecological and evolutionary success (Wilson 1971, 1985; Hölldobler and Wilson 1990). Within a colony, reproduction is allocated to only one or a few highly fertile individuals. Non-reproductive workers perform colony maintenance tasks, such as brood-care, colony defense, and foraging (Oster and Wilson 1978). Non-reproductive division of labor may be associated with variation in body size and morphology, differences in genotype, experience, or social feedback (Jeanson and Weidenmüller 2014). In addition, many eusocial insect species also display temporal or age-based polyethism, wherein workers successively pass through phases of performing specific tasks as they age (Oster and Wilson 1978; Robinson 1992; Beshers and Fewell 2001). Young individuals typically focus on tasks in the nest and switch to working outside when they are older (Wilson 1971; Oster and Wilson 1978). This transition involves considerable behavioral, transcriptomic, and physiological changes, including altered hormone and protein levels, a decline in immunity, and accelerated aging (Robinson 1987; Page and Peng 2001; Amdam et al. 2004; Giray et al. 2005; Smith et al. 2011; Bernadou et al. 2015). Most of these changes seem to be reversible to some extent, and the option to accelerate or delay the behavioral development of workers might help colonies to more easily adapt to changes in colony demography and environmental conditions (Robinson 1992). However, young workers forced to forage prematurely and older foragers returning to nursing are often less efficient (Robinson et al. 1992; Toth and Robinson 2005; Amdam et al. 2005; Page and Amdam 2007; Giehr et al. 2017). In any case, the age at the transition from inside to outside tasks represents an important regulator of colony dynamics and performance, and on the individual level it strongly influences residual lifespan of the worker (Sakagami and Fukuda 1968; Guzmán-Novoa et al. 1994; Rueppell et al. 2007, 2008, 2009; Santana Vieira et al. 2010; Kwapich and Tschinkel 2016).

The ponerine ant Platythyrea punctata (Formicidae) is a suitable system for investigations about what proximately underlies division of labor among monomorphic individuals and the consequences of division of labor on lifespan (Bernadou et al. 2015, 2018). In most studied populations of this ant, colonies do not contain mated queens or gamergates (mated workers). Instead, all unmated workers are in principle capable of producing female offspring from unfertilized eggs by thelytokous parthenogenesis (Heinze and Hölldobler 1995; Brunner et al. 2009). Young workers establish rank orders through aggressive behaviors such as antennal boxing and biting (Heinze and Hölldobler 1995; Hartmann et al. 2003). In each colony only one or occasionally a few dominant workers begin to lay eggs, while all other workers refrain from reproduction. Reproductive workers live significantly longer than their coeval non-reproductive nestmates (Hartmann and Heinze 2003). Division of labor among non-reproductive workers appears to follow a typical age-based polyethism (Hartmann and Heinze 2003; Bernadou et al. 2015). Foragers are usually older than nurses but at least some of them are able to revert to nursing and egg laying. This reversal appears to slow down their senescence relative to other individuals of the same age (Bernadou et al. 2015). Beyond these two studies, however, few data are available on task partitioning through the lifespan of P. punctata individuals, and detailed information about the pace of behavioral transitions is still lacking.

Though insect societies have been studied for many decades, there is still a lack of primary sociometric data for many species (Tschinkel 1991; Hartmann et al. 2003; Brunner et al. 2009; Kellner et al. 2013). Here, we therefore document in detail the daily activity patterns of P. punctata colonies and age-associated behavioral changes in workers. We investigated how chronological age affects task allocation and how life-history components (i.e. lifespan, age of first foraging, and foraging span) interacted among them. In particular, we tested how the transition to foraging affects the residual lifespan of individual workers (Guzmán-Novoa et al. 1994; Rueppell et al. 2007, 2008, 2009).

Material and Methods

Study Species and Colony Maintenance

Colonies of Platythyrea punctata were collected in Puerto Rico in 2012 (PR09: Parcelas Vieques, 18°25'9.01"N, 65°49'42.34"W; PR25: Luquillo, 18°22'53.83"N, 65°43'41.13"W; PR42: Yuquyiu, 18°20'48.78"N, 65°43'47.76"W, see below). Colonies have since been maintained in the laboratory either at room temperature (23–26 °C with a 12 h light:dark cycle for the experiment on age-based division of labor, lifespan and its components, see below) or in climate chambers (22–26 °C with a 12 h light:dark cycle for the experiment on activity patterns) in plastic boxes (L x W x H: 20 cm x 20 cm x 9 cm) with a plaster floor, which was regularly moistened. Chambers dug in the plaster and covered by glass plates and black plastic film served as nests. Colonies were fed with diluted honey, small cockroaches or fruit flies three times per week. Water was provided ad libitum in a plastic tube plugged with cotton wool (Bernadou et al. 2018). Prolonged lab rearing did not seem to have visible effect on colony behavior.

Activity Patterns of Colonies

To determine the activity levels of colonies, we calculated the mean percentage of ants found outside the nests of nine colonies every hour over a four-day period (mean colony size [CI95%]: 83.44 [63.57-103.31] workers). One hour before starting the experiment, colonies were fed with diluted honey, cockroaches or fruit flies, and the plaster was moistened (colonies were fed only once over the four-day period). To easily assess the number of ants outside the nest on the pictures, all debris (dead ants and cockroaches / Drosophila carcasses from previous feeding) were removed, the black plastic film covering the nest was replaced by a red one and the water tube was replaced by humidified cotton wool placed in a small Petri dish (the cotton was regularly moistened to provide water ad libitum). Colony activity was recorded with a camera (Panasonic® LUMIX model DMC-FZ1000, autofocus mode) fixed on an adjustable stand and centered 75 cm above the colony. All experiments started around 12:00 i.e., four hours after temperatures had increased from night to day conditions, and pictures were taken every 20 min over a period of four days (3 pictures per hour, 216 pictures in total per colony). At night, photos were taken with an optical flash. For two colonies, we missed one and 36 pictures due to erroneous programming of the camera. For each picture, we counted the number of individuals found outside the nest and calculated, based on colony size, the mean percentage of individuals outside for each hour.

Age-based Division of Labor, Lifespan and its Components

Eight to nine callow workers from the three different colonies (mean colony size [CI95%]; PR09: 201.12 [190.51-211.73]; PR25: 166.5 [164.6-168.4] and PR42: 96.86 [85.45-108.27] workers) were individually marked with Edding® paint markers on the day of hatching. The observations started one to three days after marking (mean [CI95%]: 1.7 [1.33–2.07] days – workers were marked but not observed over the weekend) to allow ants to recover from handling. Subsequently, the location (inside or outside the nest) and the behavior of all marked individuals were recorded by scan sampling (37 types of behavior; Bernadou et al. 2015). The scan sampling sessions were carried out randomly between 09:00 and 20:00 (minimum time interval between two consecutive scans of the same worker: 30 minutes). We observed each ant three times per day and five days per week (from Monday to Friday). After 12 weeks, we recorded only the location of marked individuals three times per day and three days per week (on Monday, Wednesday and Friday) until a worker’s death. Data on the location of workers during the first 6 weeks were collected and presented elsewhere (Appendix 2, Bernadou et al. 2015).

For each worker, we calculated three life-history components: lifespan (time from when a worker hatched until its death, in days), age of first foraging (first time a worker was observed outside the nest, in days), and foraging span (first time a worker was observed outside the nest until its death, in days) (Guzmán-Novoa et al. 1994; Becerra-Guzmán et al. 2005; Rueppell et al. 2007).

Statistical Analyses

Activity Patterns of Colonies

The mean activity pattern of colonies per hour was plotted over the four days. The mean percentages of individuals found outside the nest per colony during the day and night periods (light:dark cycles for day 2 and 3) or for the entire days 2 and 3 were compared by paired t-tests. Days 1 and 4 were excluded from the statistical analysis as we recorded only 12 hours for each day.

Age-based Division of Labor, Lifespan and its Components

To visually asses the behavioral changes with age, the most prominent behavioral acts were grouped into six categories (Bernadou et al. 2015: see ethogram – Appendix 1): brood care (except “lay eggs”), aggression, allogrooming (grooming a nestmate or being groomed), self-directed behaviors (“self-grooming,” “feed on prey or honey,” and “drink or chew cotton plug water,” Londe et al. 2016), inactivity (“inactivity on brood” and “quiescent”) and walking (“walk or move”). Infrequent behaviors were excluded from the analysis. The mean percentage of performing a task per colony was calculated for each week and plotted for the first 12 weeks.

The percentage of time workers were found outside the nest per week was analyzed by a linear mixed model (LME, lmer function, lme4 package: Bates et al. 2015). The factors “weeks” (linear and quadratic terms), “colony” and their interaction were introduced in the model as explanatory variables. We included a linear and a quadratic term to model for linear and nonlinear changes in the time spent outside the nest with age. As we repeatedly observed the same workers over time, we included the term “worker identity” as a random factor.

We tested how life-history components (i.e., lifespan, age of first foraging and foraging span) interacted among them. We built three different Poisson generalized linear models (GLMs) and tested whether the factors “colony,” “age of first foraging” (or “foraging span”), and their interaction influenced the lifespan (or the foraging span) of individuals. Because of overdispersion, the standard errors were corrected using a quasi-GLM model (Zuur et al. 2009).

Statistical analyses were performed by the software R 3.5.3 (R Development Core Team 2016). The least non-significant terms (P > 0.05) were successively removed and we compared the nested models by likelihood ratio tests to obtain minimal models. The P values for the final models were obtained by the function Anova from the “car” package (Fox and Weisberg 2011). The assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity were checked by Shapiro–Wilk test of normality and Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance respectively. Data were transformed when necessary.

Results

Activity Patterns of Colonies

The percentage of workers found outside the nest remained stable and high for two colonies over the whole observation period (mean [CI95%] percentage of workers found outside the nest over four days: 55.25 [53.69–56.81] and 59.34 [58.25–60.43] %; Fig. S1). These latter two colonies were therefore excluded from subsequent analysis (Fig. 1 and Fig. S1). For the seven remaining colonies a clear activity pattern was observed: workers were mostly active during day time with a peak activity from 13:00 to 19:00 (Fig. 1). The percentage of workers found outside the nest was significantly higher during day than night times (mean [CI95%] percentage of workers found outside the nest from 08 h to 20 h vs. 20 h-08h: 20.91 [15.48–26.34] vs. 12.30 [0.01–24.59] and 28.63 [24.4-32.86] vs. 16.28 [10.46–22.1] for days 2 and 3 respectively; Fig. 1) (paired t-test, t = 3.77, P = 0.009 and t = 4.73, P = 0.003, N = 07, for days 2 and 3 respectively).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Percentage of Platythyrea punctata workers found outside their nest per hour over four days. The thin grey lines represent the mean number of workers found outside per colony and per hour (N = 07 colonies tested, two additional colonies were discarded; see text and Supplementary material, Figure S1). The bold black line represents the mean number of workers found outside the nest over all colonies and per hour. Colony activity was recorded by taking pictures every 20 min. For each picture, the number of individuals found outside the nest was counted and we calculated, based on colony size, the mean percentage of individuals outside at each hour. The hatched areas represent night periods

We found also a significant effect of time since the last feeding on the number of workers found outside the nest: colonies allocated more workers outside on day 3 than on day 2 (mean [CI95%] percentage of workers found outside the nest: 16.37 [10.69–22.05] and 21.08 [17-25.16] % for days 2 and 3 respectively; Fig. 1) (paired t-test day 2 vs. day 3, t = -2.96, P = 0.02, N = 07).

Age-based Division of Labor

Two workers died one and eight days after the marking probably due to handling and were therefore excluded from subsequent analyses. Over the 12 weeks of observation, individually marked workers exhibited a typical age-based polyethism (Fig. 2). Among the six behavioral categories defined, we found that the percentages of time spent with brood care, aggression, and walking varied with age. Within all three colonies, the percentage of brood care and aggression decreased consistently with time while that of walking increased (Fig. 2). Though the pattern was less clear for all colonies, self-directed behaviors, such as self-grooming or eating, decreased until the 5th to 6th week and remained nearly constant afterwards. Over the 12 weeks, workers spent much time inactive, but neither for inactivity nor for allogrooming did we observe any clear tendencies (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean percentage frequency of Platythyrea punctata workers from three different colonies performing a certain task over a period of 12 weeks (the colors light grey, dark grey and black represent the colonies PR09, PR25 and PR42, respectively). To simplify graphical presentation, we plotted the mean percentage per week per colony

Overall, the percentage of time individuals spent outside the nest increased significantly and linearly over time (LME: Χ21 = 50.47, P < 0.001; Fig. 3A). However, the quadratic effect of week showed a non-significant trend towards predicting the time spent outside the nest (LME: Χ21 = 2.92, P = 0.09). The interaction between weeks and colony (LME: Χ22 = 13.39, P < 0.01; Fig. 3A) and between the quadratic effect of weeks and colony (LME: Χ22 = 15.68, P < 0.001; Fig. 3A) were also significant. The factor colony alone did not influence the percentage of time individuals spent outside the nest (LME: Χ22 = 2.32, P = 0.31).

Fig. 3
figure 3

A Percentage of time per week individual Platythyrea punctata workers were found outside the nest over 40 weeks. Eight to nine individuals from three different colonies (the colors light grey, dark grey and black represent the colonies PR09, PR25 and PR42 respectively) were used. To simplify graphical presentation, we plotted the percentage of time per week per colony with untransformed data. The percentage of time individuals spent outside the nest was significantly influenced by week (LME: Χ22 = 50.47, P < 0.001), by the interaction between weeks and colony (LME: Χ22 = 13.39, P < 0.01) and between the quadratic effect of weeks and colony (LME: Χ22 = 15.68, P < 0.001). Models were run using transformed data (see text). B Lifespan of P. punctata workers from three different colonies plotted against the age of the first foraging trip. Survival was influenced by colony (GLM: Χ22 = 74.46, P < 0.001) and by the age of the first foraging trip (GLM: Χ21 = 5.12, P = 0.02)

Age of Foraging, Foraging Span and Lifespan

The lifespan of P. punctata workers was significantly influenced by the age at which workers were observed foraging for the first time (GLM: Χ21 = 5.12, P = 0.02; see Fig. 3B) and by colony (GLM: Χ22 = 74.46, P < 0.001) but not by the interaction between the two factors (GLM: Χ22 = 3.96, P = 0.14).

The total lifespan of workers was significantly associated with foraging span (GLM: Χ21 = 138.69, P < 0.001), colony origin (GLM: Χ22 = 15.25, P < 0.001), and their interaction (GLM: Χ22 = 18.14, P < 0.001).

Finally, we found a significant effect of colony (GLM: Χ22 = 89.09, P < 0.001) but no effects of age of first foraging (GLM: Χ21 = 0.53, P = 0.47) and the interaction between age of first foraging and colony (GLM: Χ22 = 2.79, P = 0.25) on the foraging span.

Discussion

Age-based polyethism has been described for numerous species of social insects (ants, e.g. Calabi et al. 1983, Nakata 1995, Camargo et al. 2007; honey bees, e.g. Michener 1974; wasps, e.g. Ross and Matthews 1991; termites, e.g. Hinze and Leuthold 1999) including the clonal ant Platythyrea punctata (Hartmann and Heinze 2003; Bernadou et al. 2015). In this study we examined in more detail daily activity patterns of colonies and division of non-reproductive labor in P. punctata, focusing on the influence of the age of first foraging on residual life span.

Though our colonies have been maintained under laboratory conditions for several years, workers display a clear activity pattern and are mostly active during the day. This is consistent with results on the Malaysian species Platythyrea parallela, which is diurnal with most individuals being active during midday (Ashikin and Hashim 2015). On the contrary, workers of the African species Platythyrea conradti show a peak of activity around dawn (Dejean 2011). Although the activity of our colonies could have been driven by the day:night temperatures of the climatic chambers, this explanation can be partly excluded (Ashikin and Hashim 2015). Colonies allocate more workers outside the nest as the time since the last feeding day increases. Therefore, night temperature does not prevent workers from foraging. Why two colonies did not show a clear daily activity pattern is unclear. A simple explanation might be that these colonies contained more workers and less space in the nest chambers than the other ones (nest chambers were approximately of the same size across the nine colonies), forcing workers to stay outside their nests.

Young workers remained in the nest, where they nursed the brood and behaved aggressively towards their nestmates (Bernadou et al. 2015, 2018). Aggression might be explained by two non-exclusive hypotheses. Most workers in P. punctata can lay eggs; however, reproduction is monopolized by one, occasionally several, dominant workers (Heinze and Hölldobler 1995; Hartmann and Heinze 2003; Bernadou et al. 2015). Therefore (1) young workers may try to aggressively secure a high position in the colony’s hierarchy (Monnin and Peeters 1999). In addition, workers may attack rival nestmates that have activated their ovaries despite the presence of a dominant reproductive (“worker policing,” Hartmann et al. 2003) and thus stabilize the division of reproductive labor. Like dominance interactions, policing may increase the aggressor’s rank in the hierarchy and thus its chance to replace a weakening established reproductive (Brunner et al. 2009). Alternatively, (2) aggressions might reflect intrinsic quality differences between workers and ensure that only the most fecund individuals contribute to a colony’s reproduction (Brunner et al. 2009). The link between social status and egg laying capacities or other intrinsic individual qualities, however, remains to be tested. After about two months of age, the frequency of brood care and aggression decreased for most workers while the frequency of walking increased. Young workers that failed to reach a top position in the hierarchy, moved towards the nest entrance and switched from tasks inside the nest to outside tasks (Hartmann and Heinze 2003; Bernadou et al. 2015).

The lifespan of non-reproductive workers was about 200 days (but see colony PR42, around 100 days; Fig. 3), which is consistent with previous estimates (Hartman and Heinze 2003). Workers that started foraging later in life reached a considerably longer total lifespan. Age of first foraging, i.e. the transition from nest activities to outside tasks, is a key determinant of worker lifespan in social insects and is likely controlled by several physiological or environmental factors (Neukirch 1982; Guzmán-Novoa et al. 1994; Rueppell et al. 2007, 2008). Our results match previous findings on honey bee workers and confirm the importance of age of first foraging also for the lifespan of ants (e.g. Guzmán-Novoa et al. 1994; Becerra-Guzmán et al. 2005; Rueppell et al. 2007, 2008). The fact, that our colonies were kept in small boxes without predation and other external mortality risks and foraging distances were short, also supports the view that the reduced residual lifespan after the beginning of foraging is not only caused by external mortality risks or wear and tear.

In honey bees, the age of first foraging is negatively correlated with foraging span. This was interpreted as “pre-foraging senescence” (Guzmán-Novoa et al. 1994; Becerra-Guzmán et al. 2005; Rueppell et al. 2007, 2008, 2009). Indeed, intranidal activities in honey bees, such as comb construction, nest maintenance, or brood care, are energetically costly and might result in lipid and protein depletion before foraging onset (Amdam and Omholt 2002; Toth and Robinson 2005; Rueppell et al. 2007). In our study, we could not document a substantial effect of the age of first foraging on foraging span: workers did not appear to trade-off foraging span with days spent inside their nest. The costs of intranidal tasks might be lower in P. punctata than in bees, e.g., brood care activities are probably more limited. We have also shown that P. punctata foragers can revert to nursing, egg laying, and regain fat content (Bernadou et al. 2015, 2020). This suggests that workers are more flexible (Bernadou et al. 2015, 2020) and that pre-foraging senescence can be reverted and is limited in comparison to honey bee workers. For example, reverted nurses in the honey bee do not rebuild their lipid stores (Toth and Robinson 2005).

Worker lifespan and the percentage of time spent outside the nest varied considerably among the colonies. Environmental effects can be excluded as colonies were maintained under controlled conditions. The influence of genetic variation on lifespan and its components, e.g. age of first foraging, has been repeatedly reported in honey bees (see e.g. Calderone and Page 1988; Guzmán-Novoa and Gary 1993; Page and Pankiw 2001; Robinson 2002; Rueppell et al. 2004) and likely occurs also in P. punctata, as different colonies belong to different clones. Colony size may be another factor that indirectly influences the longevity of workers and its components. On the one hand, in smaller colonies, young workers might forage earlier but also have higher workloads, which might negatively impact their survival (Winston and Fergusson 1985; O’Donnell and Jeanne 1992; but see Wolf and Schmid-Hempel 1990; Rueppell et al. 2009). On the other hand, large colony size may favor worker specialization and maximizes colony efficiency (Oster and Wilson 1978; Karsai and Wenzel 1998; Gautrais et al. 2002; Holbrook et al. 2011; but see Dornhaus et al. 2009). Our data support this view: workers from the smallest colony (PR42) started foraging earlier and had a shorter lifespan than workers from the largest colony (PR09). In contrast to colonies PR09 and PR42, the results for colony PR25 are more puzzling: age of first foraging was not associated with total worker longevity. This might be due to the percentage of time workers spent outside the nest, i.e., while it continuously increased in the other two colonies, time spent outside stabilized over time in PR25. It is tempting to speculate that more time spent inside the nest reduced outdoor activities, increased fat content of workers and slowed down senescence (Bernadou et al. 2015, 2020).

In conclusion, we confirmed and documented daily activity pattern of P. punctata and age-associated behavioral changes in workers. However, this age-based polyethism as well as lifespan and its components seem to be highly influenced by colony origin. Whether colony size or genetic background controls these variations needs further investigation.