Elsevier

Displays

Volume 61, January 2020, 101932
Displays

With flying colours: Pilot performance with colour-coded head-up flight symbology

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.displa.2019.101932Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Colour head-up symbology improves manual flying performance.

  • Equivalent colour feedback performance benefits for non-pilot and expert pilots.

  • No perceived subjective workload benefit of colour coded feedback.

  • Usability preference of colour symbology vs. monochrome symbology.

Abstract

The manipulation of colour in display symbology design has long been recognised as a method to improve operator experience and performance. Recent developments in colour head-up display (HUD) and helmet-mounted display (HMD) technology underline the necessity to understand the human factors considerations of symbology colour coding against conventional monochrome symbology formats. In this low-fidelity desktop human-in-the-loop experiment, the colour of flight symbology on an overlaid symbology set was coded as a redundant cue to indicate the accuracy of professional and non-professional pilots’ flight profile across a range of simulated flight manoeuvres. The main finding of this study was that colour coding flight symbology supported the manual flying performance of both professional and non-professional pilots. Notably, colour-coding of the bank indicator and airspeed tape minimised performance error during turning and altitude change manoeuvres, respectively. The usability of colour coded symbology was also rated higher than the monochrome symbology. We conclude that colour coded HUD/HMD symbology is preferred by the user and may improve performance during low workload manual flying tasks. A fuller understanding of performance and workload effects will require future studies to employ higher workload flying tasks and examine the utility of colour coding within higher fidelity environments.

Introduction

The conscious attentional effort required to locate and scrutinise target information in a busy visual scene can be minimised by pre-attentive processing when the target stimuli differs from noncritical information on a single dimension [1], [2], [3], [4], [5]. When pre-attentive processing is successful in this manner the target should “pop-out” of the display [6]. An example would be the search of a single red item among a set of green distractor items. In this case, the red target pops out and summons attention with minimal interference from the green distractors. When symbology varies along multiple dimensions (e.g. colour and shape), pre-attentive processing may be able to isolate a group of likely target candidates based on one dimension (e.g. colour), but then explicit attentional resources are required to guide attention over the reduced symbology set (e.g. shapes of a specific colour). Evidently, performance will be much slower in this instance since explicit attention is required to scrutinise individual items within the reduced symbology set in serial fashion until the target is located [7], [8], [9]. Nevertheless, performance is still superior to the case where there is no colour coding. Because it is so effective, selection by colour is a common dimension of symbology that has been manipulated in the design of visual displays to improve operator experience and performance [10], [11], [12].

The above principles have been successful applied to the design of cockpit displays to improve the communication of safety critical information for more than 75 years [13]. The Federal Aviation Administration (FAA) advises that in addition to utilising visually distinct colour sets, colours in electronic flight displays must be employed only as a redundant cue and be semantically standardised (FAA Advisory circular: 25-11B [14]). For example, the progression from green to red is commonly used to semantically convey increasing degrees of threat, a potential hazard, safety criticality, or the need for flight crew awareness and/or response. There is strong body evidence that has highlighted that colour can be used to support cognitive functions, improve pilot spatial orientation, enhance accuracy, decision time and workload [13], [15], [16], [17]. In the military domain, colour has been used to support the identification of targets, smoke, flags, signal and navigation lights, and terrain differences [18]. In the commercial world, colour coding has been employed within TCAS modes of head-down navigational displays (ND) to support discrimination between TCAS proximity cautions and alerts. Future avionic applications such as the Airborne Separation Assurance System (ASAS) will utilise colour within head-down Primary Flight Displays (PFD) and ND to support pilots maintain self-assured separation during free-routing operations [19], [20]. Specifically, yellow or orange coloured “no-go” bands placed on the vertical speed tape and heading rose would represent potential conflicts between 3 and 6 min away, respectively.

Head-up displays (HUDs) and Helmet-Mounted Displays (HMDs) allow pilots to see key flight instrumentation on a transparent display whilst maintaining their view of the outside world. To achieve this both technologies optimally superimposed the symbology of the transparent display onto the user’s field of view. This collation of near and far flight information removes the need to look down at the flight instruments, resulting in increased situational awareness and greater precision in aircraft control [21]. HUD and HMD imagery is often restricted to monochrome (green) as a consequence of the single P-53 phosphor that is used to generate the imagery [22]. This results in the omission of information normally provided, or organised, by colour coding. However, recent advancements in waveguide optical technology means that the development of colour HUDs could be viable in the near future [23]. The display technology in HMDs is different and the development and design of colour displays has matured further in comparison to HUDs. For example, the United Force Airforce (USAF) has addressed several relevant HMD visual processing issues such as the appropriate luminance contrasts ratios for a colour HMD [24], [25]. Nonetheless, due to their complexity and high cost, colour HUDs and HMDs have been late in development [26]. Consequently, the related human factors considerations of colour have been largely ignored. This is reflected in the absence of specific colour guidance from the FAA regarding the presentation of information on HUDs (FAA Advisory circular: 25-11B (Federal Aviation Administration, 2014)). However, these factors have not decreased their desirability to the user [16]. With the possibility to develop more visually distinct and complex head-up and head-mounted display systems in the near future an understanding of relevant human factors has become more urgent.

Several studies have confirmed the positive impact of colour on flight performance and operations. DeMars (1975) concluded that, for certain applications, colour enhanced accuracy, decision time, and workload capability. In a study by Derefeldt et al. [27], an upgraded military colour coded head-down display was discovered to provide more target search and tracking advantages than the earlier monochrome display. Furthermore, the colour displays reduced reaction times and helped pilots to see the grouping of information on the display. Similarly, colour-coding weapon symbology of military pilot HMDs can reduce missile release time without sacrificing probability of kill [28]. Conversely, Dudfield [29], [36] found that the performance benefits of colour-coded flight symbology on a HUD far outweighed its perceived importance. However, the pilots in Dudfield’s study noted that the difficultly of the employed task, maintenance of a straight-level profile, was not sufficiently challenging.

The intention of the current study is to evaluate the performance and workload benefits of a colour coded head-up flight symbology set. Rather than creating a physical HUD or HMD platform we decided to present an artificial overlay on a computer screen that would in essence create an “artificial HUD”. This of course creates an offset in terms of visual acuity and human performance, but the primary focus of this study was to examine the cognitive effects of colour (and not assess the focal demands of the display). Manual flying performance and subjective workload of professional commercial pilots and non-professional pilots was examined in response to flying with a redundantly colour coded flight symbology set across a range of low-fidelity simulated flight trials. Similar to Dudfield (1991), symbology colour coding cues were based on economy so that colour was used only when participants flew outside pre-determined boundaries, e.g. flying off course, providing the subject with immediate feedback on the accuracy of their performance. However, we expanded on Dudfield’s study by evaluating the use of colour feedback across several flight manoeuvre types (ranging in complexity), not just straight-and-level flight. The use of colour in display design has been used frequently to facilitate learning [30], the inclusion of inexperienced participants in the current study was to determine whether the availability of the colour-coded redundant information served a cognitive purpose beyond facilitating the learning process in a novice group. In addition, subjective measures of workload and usability were measured via the NASA Task Load Index (TLX) and the Post-Study System Usability Questionnaire (PSSUQ), respectively.

Section snippets

Participants

Nine professional commercial airline pilots and eighteen non-professional pilots participated in the study. The rank of professional pilot participants included three first officers and six captains. Pilots’ average flying experience was 5550 h (SD = 4149). Non-pilots consisted of volunteer aviation/automotive engineering students and staff from the Faculty of Mobility and Transport at Coventry University. The experiment was approved by Coventry University Ethics and was in line with Ethical

Indicated air speed (IAS)

Mean RMSE IAS performance, with standard deviations, is presented in Fig. 2. For non-pilots, mean IAS RMSE clearly decreased when colour feedback was provided (RMSE mean difference: Straight and Level (S&L) = 0.14; Climbing Turn = 0.86; Flat Turn = 1.04; Descent = 1.74). Similarly, mean IAS RMSE was lower with colour feedback for pilots, but to a lesser extent, on S&L (RMSE mean difference: = 0.26), climbing turns (0.25) and descents (0.44). Comparison of AIC values of main effect and

Discussion

The current study aim was to investigate if benefits provided by colour in head-down displays could be extended to different types of head up displays. Specifically, we evaluated the utility of a head-up colour coded symbology set to support the manual flying performance of professional commercial pilots and non-pilots.

As shown in this human-in-the-loop study, the performance of both professional and non-professional pilots benefitted from the presence of a colour coded symbology set.

Conclusion and recommendations

The main finding of this study was that head-up colour coded symbology improved the performance of both professional commercial pilots and non-professional pilots during a low workload manual flying task. Specifically, colour coding of the bank indicator and airspeed tape minimised error during turning and altitude change manoeuvres, respectively. In this context, such a finding has not been previously reported. Despite the absence of a colour-related workload improvement, the results of this

Funding

This work was supported by Innovate UK, the UK’s Innovation Agency, with support from the UK Aerospace Technology Institute (grant reference number 113108).

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Acknowledgements

This article is based on work performed in the programme: Open Flight Deck, which has received funding from Innovate UK programme, the UK’s Innovation Agency, with support from the UK Aerospace Technology Institute (grant reference number 113108). The views and opinions expressed in this paper are those of the authors and are not intended to represent the position or opinions of the Open Flight Deck consortium or any of the individual partner organisations.

References (45)

  • J.M. Wolfe, Visual Attention, in: Seeing, Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2000, pp....
  • J. Blundell, S. Frisson, A. Chakrapani, S. Kearney, S. Vijay, A. Macdonald, P. Gissen, C. Hendriksz, Markers of...
  • K. Nakayama et al.

    Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions

    Nature

    (1986)
  • U. Neisser

    Visual search

    Sci. Am.

    (1964)
  • R.C. Carter

    Visual search with color

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1982)
  • W. Schneider et al.

    Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention

    Psychol. Rev.

    (1977)
  • R.W. Backs et al.

    Ocular measures of redundancy gain during visual search of colour symbolic displays

    Ergonomics

    (1995)
  • N. Dahlstrom et al.

    Fidelity and validity of simulator training

    Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci.

    (2009)
  • K.F. van Orden et al.

    Redundant use of luminance and flashing with shape and color as highlighting codes in symbolic displays

    Hum. Factors

    (1993)
  • J.E. Melzer, K.W. Moffitt, Color Helmet Display for the Military Cockpit, in: Proc. IEEE/AIAA 11th Digit. Avion. Syst....
  • Federal Aviation Administration, Advisory Circular AC 25-11B Transport Category Airplane Electronic Flight Displays,...
  • S. DeMars, Human factors considerations for the use of color in display systems, Sci. Tech. Aerosp. Reports, 1975, p....
  • Cited by (7)

    • Effects of input method and display mode of situation map on early warning aircraft reconnaissance task performance with different information complexities

      2023, Chinese Journal of Aeronautics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Color-coding of the situation map has generally been recognized as an effective means to improve task performance.15 Previous studies have found that color-coding reduced target search time16,17 and workload in flight tasks compared to monochrome displays.18 Yeh and Wickens reported that the coding of differential classes facilitated performance and reduced the negative influence of clutter.19

    • Urgency-Based color coding to support visual search in displays for supervisory control of multiple unmanned aircraft systems

      2022, Displays
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, it is necessary to develop means to efficiently guide the remote pilot’s visual attention to the indications of critical system states [15]. Future displays should enable the remote pilot to effortlessly perceive the current state of each safety critical system function, e.g. through icon-based interfaces [16,17]. Friedrich and Lieb and Friedrich and Vollrath present icon-based Human Machine Interfaces (HMIs) supporting a remote pilot to monitor safety critical information associated with operating multiple UAS [18,19].

    • Moving and improving in safety-critical systems: Impacts of head-mounted displays on operator mobility, performance, and situation awareness

      2021, International Journal of Human Computer Studies
      Citation Excerpt :

      These results are important considering the scarcity of work measuring the impact of HMDs on operator mobility, performance, and SA in safety-critical systems where operator mobility is required. The H2 results are aligned with prior work for stationary operators in safety-critical systems (Blundell et al., 2020; Follmann et al., 2019; Stanton et al., 2019; Stewart and Billinghurst, 2016) and now contribute to our understanding of HMD impacts on operators in safety-critical systems where mobility is required. Operator mobility was significantly enhanced with HMDs, suggesting that access to relevant information from distributed sources while mobile enhanced an operator's ability to effectively perform tasks that require mobility.

    • Workload benefits of colour coded head-up flight symbology during high workload flight

      2020, Displays
      Citation Excerpt :

      Dudfield [22] found that colour-coded error feedback provided on HUD flight symbology did not support pilot flying accuracy during straight and level flight, but did benefit subjective workload. Findings from our own recent study [23] demonstrated that a similar colour-coded head up symbology set granted minor and major flying accuracy enhancements to professional airline pilots and novices, respectively. It is likely that the performance benefits found in our study arose from the more varied range of manoeuvres, such as climbing turns, that required pilots to attend to both dynamic and static flight parameters.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This paper was recommended for publication by Richard H.Y. So.

    View full text