Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Perspective
  • Published:

Beyond dichotomies in reinforcement learning

Abstract

Reinforcement learning (RL) is a framework of particular importance to psychology, neuroscience and machine learning. Interactions between these fields, as promoted through the common hub of RL, has facilitated paradigm shifts that relate multiple levels of analysis in a singular framework (for example, relating dopamine function to a computationally defined RL signal). Recently, more sophisticated RL algorithms have been proposed to better account for human learning, and in particular its oft-documented reliance on two separable systems: a model-based (MB) system and a model-free (MF) system. However, along with many benefits, this dichotomous lens can distort questions, and may contribute to an unnecessarily narrow perspective on learning and decision-making. Here, we outline some of the consequences that come from overconfidently mapping algorithms, such as MB versus MF RL, with putative cognitive processes. We argue that the field is well positioned to move beyond simplistic dichotomies, and we propose a means of refocusing research questions towards the rich and complex components that comprise learning and decision-making.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: RL across fields of research.
Fig. 2: Contrast between MB and MF algorithms in response to environmental changes.
Fig. 3: Decompositions of learning.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Roiser, J. P. & Sahakian, B. J. Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr. 18, 139–149 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Dickinson, A. Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London. B Biol. Sci. 308, 67–78 (1985).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Sloman, S. A. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 119, 3 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69, 1204–1215 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate? Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 645–665 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kahneman, D. & Frederick, S. in Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment Ch. 2 (eds Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. & Kahneman, D.) 49–81 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2002).

  7. Daw, N. in Decision Making, Affect, and Learning: Attention and Performance XXIII Ch. 1 (eds Delgado, M. R., Phelps, E. A. and Robbins, T. W.) 1–26 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2011).

  8. Marr, D. & Poggio, T. A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 204, 301–328 (1979).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Doll, B. B., Duncan, K. D., Simon, D. A., Shohamy, D. & Daw, N. D. Model-based choices involve prospective neural activity. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 767–772 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Daw, N. D. Are we of two minds? Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1497–1499 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Dayan, P. Goal-directed control and its antipodes. Neural Netw. 22, 213–219 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. da Silva, C. F. & Hare, T. A. A note on the analysis of two-stage task results: how changes in task structure affect what model-free and model-based strategies predict about the effects of reward and transition on the stay probability. PLoS ONE 13, e0195328 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Moran, R., Keramati, M., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. Retrospective model-based inference guides model-free credit assignment. Nat. Commun. 10, 750 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Akam, T., Costa, R. & Dayan, P. Simple plans or sophisticated habits? State, transition and learning interactions in the two-step task. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11, e1004648 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Shahar, N. et al. Credit assignment to state-independent task representations and its relationship with model-based decision making. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 15871–15876 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Deserno, L. & Hauser, T. U. Beyond a cognitive dichotomy: can multiple decision systems prove useful to distinguish compulsive and impulsive symptom dimensions? Biol. Psychiatry https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.03.004 (2020).

  17. Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Dabney, W. et al. A distributional code for value in dopamine-based reinforcement learning. Nature 577, 671–675 (2020).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Thorndike, E. L. Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies (Transaction, 1965).

  20. Bush, R. R. & Mosteller, F. Stochastic models for learning (John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 1955).

  21. Pearce, J. M. & Hall, G. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol. Rev. 87, 532–552 (1980).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. in Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory Ch. 3 (eds Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F) 64–99 (Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972).

  23. Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A. G. Reinforcement learning: An Introduction (MIT Press, 2018).

  24. Montague, P. R., Dayan, P. & Sejnowski, T. J. A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. J. Neurosci. 16, 1936–1947 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Bayer, H. M. & Glimcher, P. W. Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron 47, 129–141 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Morris, G., Nevet, A., Arkadir, D., Vaadia, E. & Bergman, H. Midbrain dopamine neurons encode decisions for future action. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1057–1063 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Roesch, M. R., Calu, D. J. & Schoenbaum, G. Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1615–1624 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Shen, W., Flajolet, M., Greengard, P. & Surmeier, D. J. Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic plasticity. Science 321, 848–851 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Steinberg, E. E. et al. A causal link between prediction errors, dopamine neurons and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 966–973 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Kim, K. M. et al. Optogenetic mimicry of the transient activation of dopamine neurons by natural reward is sufficient for operant reinforcement. PLoS ONE 7, e33612 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. O’Doherty, J. et al. Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304, 452–454 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. McClure, S. M., Berns, G. S. & Montague, P. R. Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 38, 339–346 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Samejima, K., Ueda, Y., Doya, K. & Kimura, M. Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science 310, 1337–1340 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Lau, B. & Glimcher, P. W. Value representations in the primate striatum during matching behavior. Neuron 58, 451–463 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C. & O’Reilly, R. C. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science 306, 1940–1943 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Frank, M. J., Moustafa, A. A., Haughey, H. M., Curran, T. & Hutchison, K. E. Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 16311–16316 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Cockburn, J., Collins, A. G. & Frank, M. J. A reinforcement learning mechanism responsible for the valuation of free choice. Neuron 83, 551–557 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Frank, M. J., O’Reilly, R. C. & Curran, T. When memory fails, intuition reigns: midazolam enhances implicit inference in humans. Psychol. Sci. 17, 700–707 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Doll, B. B., Hutchison, K. E. & Frank, M. J. Dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in susceptibility to confirmation bias. J. Neurosci. 31, 6188–6198 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Doll, B. B. et al. Reduced susceptibility to confirmation bias in schizophrenia. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 715–728 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Berridge, K. C. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology 191, 391–431 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hamid, A. A. et al. Mesolimbic dopamine signals the value of work. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 117–126 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sharpe, M. J. et al. Dopamine transients are sufficient and necessary for acquisition of model-based associations. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 735–742 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Tolman, E. C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55, 189–208 (1948).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Economides, M., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Lübbert, A., Guitart-Masip, M. & Dolan, R. J. Model-based reasoning in humans becomes automatic with training. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11, e1004463 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Otto, A. R., Raio, C. M., Chiang, A., Phelps, E. A. & Daw, N. D. Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 20941–20946 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Wunderlich, K., Smittenaar, P. & Dolan, R. J. Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior. Neuron 75, 418–424 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Deserno, L. et al. Ventral striatal dopamine reflects behavioral and neural signatures of model-based control during sequential decision making. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 1595–1600 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Gillan, C. M., Otto, A. R., Phelps, E. A. & Daw, N. D. Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 523–536 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Groman, S. M., Massi, B., Mathias, S. R., Lee, D. & Taylor, J. R. Model-free and model-based influences in addiction-related behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 936–945 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Doll, B. B., Simon, D. A. & Daw, N. D. The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 1075–1081 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Cushman, F. & Morris, A. Habitual control of goal selection in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 201506367 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  53. O’Reilly, R. C. & Frank, M. J. Making working memory work: a computational model of learning in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Neural Comput. 18, 283–328 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Collins, A. G. & Frank, M. J. Cognitive control over learning: creating, clustering, and generalizing task-set structure. Psychol. Rev. 120, 190–229 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Momennejad, I. et al. The successor representation in human reinforcement learning. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 680–692 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Da Silva, C. F. & Hare, T. A. Humans are primarily model-based and not model-free learners in the two-stage task. bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/682922 (2019).

  57. Toyama, A., Katahira, K. & Ohira, H. Biases in estimating the balance between model-free and model-based learning systems due to model misspecification. J. Math. Psychol. 91, 88–102 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  58. Iigaya, K., Fonseca, M. S., Murakami, M., Mainen, Z. F. & Dayan, P. An effect of serotonergic stimulation on learning rates for rewards apparent after long intertrial intervals. Nat. Commun. 9, 2477 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Mohr, H. et al. Deterministic response strategies in a trial-and-error learning task. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14, e1006621 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Hampton, A. N., Bossaerts, P. & O’Doherty, J. P. The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in abstract state-based inference during decision making in humans. J. Neurosci. 26, 8360–8367 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Boorman, E. D., Behrens, T. E. & Rushworth, M. F. Counterfactual choice and learning in a neural network centered on human lateral frontopolar cortex. PLoS Biol. 9, e1001093 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Behrens, T. E., Woolrich, M. W., Walton, M. E. & Rushworth, M. F. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1214–1221 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Collins, A. G. E. & Koechlin, E. Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS Biol. 10, e1001293 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Gershman, S. J., Norman, K. A. & Niv, Y. Discovering latent causes in reinforcement learning. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 5, 43–50 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  65. Badre, D., Kayser, A. S. & Esposito, M. D. Article frontal cortex and the discovery of abstract action rules. Neuron 66, 315–326 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Konovalov, A. & Krajbich, I. Mouse tracking reveals structure knowledge in the absence of model-based choice. Nat. Commun. 11, 1893 (2020).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Gläscher, J., Daw, N., Dayan, P. & O’Doherty, J. P. States versus rewards: dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron 66, 585–595 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Huys, Q. J. et al. Interplay of approximate planning strategies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 3098–3103 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Suzuki, S., Cross, L. & O’Doherty, J. P. Elucidating the underlying components of food valuation in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1786 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  70. Badre, D., Doll, B. B., Long, N. M. & Frank, M. J. Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and individual differences in uncertainty-driven exploration. Neuron 73, 595–607 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Wilson, R. C., Geana, A., White, J. M., Ludvig, E. A. & Cohen, J. D. Humans use directed and random exploration to solve the explore–exploit dilemma. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 2074 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Otto, A. R., Gershman, S. J., Markman, A. B. & Daw, N. D. The curse of planning: dissecting multiple reinforcement-learning systems by taxing the central executive. Psychol. Sci. 24, 751–761 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Niv, Y. et al. Reinforcement learning in multidimensional environments relies on attention mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 35, 8145–8157 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Badre, D. & Frank, M. J. Mechanisms of hierarchical reinforcement learning in cortico-striatal circuits 2: evidence from fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 22, 527–536 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Collins, A. G. E. Reinforcement learning: bringing together computation and cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 63–68 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  76. Collins, A. G. in Goal-directed Decision Making (eds Morris, R., Bornstein, A. & Shenhav, A) 105–123 (Elsevier, 2018).

  77. Donoso, M., Collins, A. G. E. & Koechlin, E. Foundations of human reasoning in the prefrontal cortex. Science 344, 1481–1486 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Wilson, R. C., Takahashi, Y. K., Schoenbaum, G. & Niv, Y. Orbitofrontal cortex as a cognitive map of task space. Neuron 81, 267–278 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Schuck, N. W., Wilson, R. & Niv, Y. in Goal-directed Decision Making (eds Morris, R., Bornstein, A. & Shenhav, A) 259–278 (Elsevier, 2018).

  80. Ballard, I. C., Wagner, A. D. & McClure, S. M. Hippocampal pattern separation supports reinforcement learning. Nat. Commun. 10, 1073 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Redish, A. D., Jensen, S., Johnson, A. & Kurth-Nelson, Z. Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling. Psychol. Rev. 114, 784 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Bouton, M. E. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn. Mem. 11, 485–494 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Rescorla, R. A. Spontaneous recovery. Learn. Mem. 11, 501–509 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. O’Reilly, R. C., Frank, M. J., Hazy, T. E. & Watz, B. PVLV: the primary value and learned value Pavlovian learning algorithm. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 31 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Gershman, S. J., Blei, D. M. & Niv, Y. Context, learning, and extinction. Psychol. Rev. 117, 197–209 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Wang, J. X. et al. Prefrontal cortex as a meta-reinforcement learning system. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 860–868 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Iigaya, K. et al. Deviation from the matching law reflects an optimal strategy involving learning over multiple timescales. Nat. Commun. 10, 1466 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Collins, A. G. E. & Frank, M. J. How much of reinforcement learning is working memory, not reinforcement learning? A behavioral, computational, and neurogenetic analysis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1024–1035 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Collins, A. G. E. The tortoise and the hare: interactions between reinforcement learning and working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1422–1432 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  90. Viejo, G., Girard, B. B., Procyk, E. & Khamassi, M. Adaptive coordination of working-memory and reinforcement learning in non-human primates performing a trial-and-error problem solving task. Behav. Brain Res. 355, 76–89 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Poldrack, R. A. et al. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature 414, 546–550 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Foerde, K. & Shohamy, D. Feedback timing modulates brain systems for learning in humans. J. Neurosci. 31, 13157–13167 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Bornstein, A. M., Khaw, M. W., Shohamy, D. & Daw, N. D. Reminders of past choices bias decisions for reward in humans. Nat. Commun. 8, 15958 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Bornstein, A. M. & Norman, K. A. Reinstated episodic context guides sampling-based decisions for reward. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 997–1003 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Vikbladh, O. M. et al. Hippocampal contributions to model-based planning and spatial memory. Neuron 102, 683–693 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Decker, J. H., Otto, A. R., Daw, N. D. & Hartley, C. A. From creatures of habit to goal-directed learners: tracking the developmental emergence of model-based reinforcement learning. Psychol. Sci. 27, 848–858 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Dickinson, A. & Balleine, B. Motivational control of goal-directed action. Anim. Learn. Behav. 22, 1–18 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  98. Balleine, B. W. & Dickinson, A. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology 37, 407–419 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Daw, N. D. & Doya, K. The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 199–204 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Friedel, E. et al. Devaluation and sequential decisions: linking goal-directed and model-based behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 587 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  101. de Wit, S. et al. Shifting the balance between goals and habits: five failures in experimental habit induction. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147, 1043–1065 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Madrigal, R. Hot vs. cold cognitions and consumers’ reactions to sporting event outcomes. J. Consum. Psychol. 18, 304–319 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  103. Peterson, E. & Welsh, M. C. in Handbook of Executive Functioning (eds Goldstein, S. & Naglieri, J. A.) 45–65 (Springer, 2014).

  104. Barch, D. M. et al. Explicit and implicit reinforcement learning across the psychosis spectrum. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126, 694–711 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Taylor, J. A., Krakauer, J. W. & Ivry, R. B. Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task. J. Neurosci. 34, 3023–3032 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Sloman, S. A. in Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment Ch. 22 (eds Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. & Kahneman D.) 379–396 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2002).

  107. Evans, J. S. B. T. in In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (eds J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish) p. 33–54 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

  108. Stanovich, K. Rationality and the Reflective Mind (Oxford Univ. Press, 2011).

  109. Dayan, P. The convergence of TD(λ) for general λ. Mach. Learn. 8, 341–362 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  110. Caplin, A. & Dean, M. Axiomatic methods, dopamine and reward prediction error. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 197–202 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. van den Bos, W., Bruckner, R., Nassar, M. R., Mata, R. & Eppinger, B. Computational neuroscience across the lifespan: promises and pitfalls. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 42–53 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Adams, R. A., Huys, Q. J. & Roiser, J. P. Computational psychiatry: towards a mathematically informed understanding of mental illness. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 87, 53–63 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Miller, K. J., Shenhav, A. & Ludvig, E. A. Habits without values. Psychol. Rev. 126, 292–311 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  114. Botvinick, M. M., Niv, Y. & Barto, A. Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: a reinforcement-learning perspective. Cognition 113, 262–280 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Konidaris, G. & Barto, A. G. in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 22 (eds Bengio, Y., Schuurmans, D., Lafferty, J. D., Williams, C. K. I. & Culotta, A.) 1015–1023 (NIPS, 2009).

  116. Konidaris, G. On the necessity of abstraction. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 1–7 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Frank, M. J. & Fossella, J. A. Neurogenetics and pharmacology of learning, motivation, and cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 133–152 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Collins, A. G. E., Cavanagh, J. F. & Frank, M. J. Human EEG uncovers latent generalizable rule structure during learning. J. Neurosci. 34, 4677–4685 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  119. Doya, K. What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex? Neural Netw. 12, 961–974 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Fermin, A. S. et al. Model-based action planning involves cortico-cerebellar and basal ganglia networks. Sci. Rep. 6, 31378 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  121. Gershman, S. J., Markman, A. B. & Otto, A. R. Retrospective revaluation in sequential decision making: a tale of two systems. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 182 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Pfeiffer, B. E. & Foster, D. J. Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals. Nature 497, 74–79 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  123. Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I. & Battaglia, F. P. Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 919–926 (2009).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  124. Collins, A. G. E., Albrecht, M. A., Waltz, J. A., Gold, J. M. & Frank, M. J. Interactions among working memory, reinforcement learning, and effort in value-based choice: a new paradigm and selective deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 431–439 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  125. Collins, A. G. E., Ciullo, B., Frank, M. J. & Badre, D. Working memory load strengthens reward prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 37, 2700–2716 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  126. Collins, A. A. G. E. & Frank, M. J. M. Within- and across-trial dynamics of human EEG reveal cooperative interplay between reinforcement learning and working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 2502–2507 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A. & Squire, L. R. A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science 273, 1399–1402 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Squire, L. R. & Zola, S. M. Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 13515–13522 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  129. Eichenbaum, H. et al. Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System (MIT Press, 1993).

  130. Foerde, K. & Shohamy, D. The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: insight from Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 624–636 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  131. Wimmer, G. E., Daw, N. D. & Shohamy, D. Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1092–1104 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  132. Wimmer, G. E., Braun, E. K., Daw, N. D. & Shohamy, D. Episodic memory encoding interferes with reward learning and decreases striatal prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 34, 14901–14912 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  133. Gershman, S. J. The successor representation: its computational logic and neural substrates. J. Neurosci. 38, 7193–7200 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  134. Kool, W., Cushman, F. A. & Gershman, S. J. in Goal-directed Decision Making Ch. 7 (eds Morris, R. W. & Bornstein, A.) 153–178 (Elsevier, 2018).

  135. Langdon, A. J., Sharpe, M. J., Schoenbaum, G. & Niv, Y. Model-based predictions for dopamine. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 49, 1–7 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Starkweather, C. K., Babayan, B. M., Uchida, N. & Gershman, S. J. Dopamine reward prediction errors reflect hidden-state inference across time. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 581–589 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  137. Krueger, K. A. & Dayan, P. Flexible shaping: how learning in small steps helps. Cognition 110, 380–394 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Bhandari, A. & Badre, D. Learning and transfer of working memory gating policies. Cognition 172, 89–100 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  139. Leong, Y. C. et al. Dynamic interaction between reinforcement learning and attention in multidimensional environments. Neuron 93, 451–463 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  140. Farashahi, S., Rowe, K., Aslami, Z., Lee, D. & Soltani, A. Feature-based learning improves adaptability without compromising precision. Nat. Commun. 8, 1768 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  141. Bach, D. R. & Dolan, R. J. Knowing how much you don’t know: a neural organization of uncertainty estimates. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 572–586 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  142. Pulcu, E. & Browning, M. The misestimation of uncertainty in affective disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 865–875 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  143. Badre, D., Frank, M. J. & Moore, C. I. Interactionist neuroscience. Neuron 88, 855–860 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  144. Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A. & Poeppel, D. Neuroscience needs behavior: correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron 93, 480–490 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  145. Doll, B. B., Shohamy, D. & Daw, N. D. Multiple memory systems as substrates for multiple decision systems. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 117, 4–13 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  146. Smittenaar, P., FitzGerald, T. H., Romei, V., Wright, N. D. & Dolan, R. J. Disruption of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex decreases model-based in favor of model-free control in humans. Neuron 80, 914–919 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  147. Doll, B. B., Bath, K. G., Daw, N. D. & Frank, M. J. Variability in dopamine genes dissociates model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. J. Neurosci. 36, 1211–1222 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  148. Voon, V. et al. Motivation and value influences in the relative balance of goal-directed and habitual behaviours in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e670 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  149. Voon, V., Reiter, A., Sebold, M. & Groman, S. Model-based control in dimensional psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 391–400 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  150. Gillan, C. M., Kosinski, M., Whelan, R., Phelps, E. A. & Daw, N. D. Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. eLife 5, e11305 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  151. Culbreth, A. J., Westbrook, A., Daw, N. D., Botvinick, M. & Barch, D. M. Reduced model-based decision-making in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 777–787 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  152. Patzelt, E. H., Kool, W., Millner, A. J. & Gershman, S. J. Incentives boost model-based control across a range of severity on several psychiatric constructs. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 425–433 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  153. Skinner, B. F. The Selection of Behavior: The Operant Behaviorism of BF Skinner: Comments and Consequences (CUP Archive, 1988).

  154. Corbit, L. H., Muir, J. L. & Balleine, B. W. Lesions of mediodorsal thalamus and anterior thalamic nuclei produce dissociable effects on instrumental conditioning in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 1286–1294 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  155. Coutureau, E. & Killcross, S. Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behav. Brain Res. 146, 167–174 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  156. Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J. & Balleine, B. W. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 181–189 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  157. Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J. & Balleine, B. W. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the action–outcome contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behav. Brain Res. 166, 189–196 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  158. Ito, M. & Doya, K. Distinct neural representation in the dorsolateral, dorsomedial, and ventral parts of the striatum during fixed-and free-choice tasks. J. Neurosci. 35, 3499–3514 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors contributed equally to all aspects of the article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne G. E. Collins.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Neuroscience thanks the anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Collins, A.G.E., Cockburn, J. Beyond dichotomies in reinforcement learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 21, 576–586 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0355-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0355-6

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing