Skip to main content
Log in

Resource Stress Predicts Changes in Religious Belief and Increases in Sharing Behavior

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examine and test alternative models for explaining the relationships between resource stress, beliefs that gods and spirits influence weather (to help or harm food supply or punish for norm violations), and customary beyond-household sharing behavior. Our model, the resource stress model, suggests that resource stress affects both sharing as well as conceptions of gods’ involvement with weather, but these supernatural beliefs play no role in explaining sharing. An alternative model, the moralizing high god model, suggests that the relationship between resource stress and sharing is at least partially mediated by religious beliefs in moralizing high gods. We compared the models using a worldwide sample of 96 cultures from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample (SCCS), newly coded data on supernatural involvement with weather, and previously coded data on food and labor sharing. We conducted three types of analysis: multilevel and society-level regressions, and mediational path modeling using Monte Carlo simulations. Resource stress shows a robust effect on beliefs that high gods are associated with weather (and the more specific beliefs that high gods help or hurt the food supply with weather), that superior gods help the food supply through weather, and that minor spirits hurt the food supply through weather. Resource stress also predicts greater belief in moralizing high gods. However, no form of high god belief that we test significantly predicts more sharing. Mediational models suggest the religious beliefs do not significantly explain why resource stress is associated with food and labor sharing. Our findings generally accord with the view that resource stress changes religious belief and has a direct effect on sharing behavior, unmediated by high god beliefs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We follow Ember and Ember (1992b) in using reliability scores of 5 or less for resource stress measures. We also excluded two societies—Hebrews and Babylonians—that did not have information on sharing from Ember et al. (2018).

  2. In response to reviewers’ comments, we subsequently reviewed the ethnographic evidence for Murdock’s (1967) codes on high gods to see if we were in agreement. After this review we removed one case (Koreans) for which there was contradictory information. The Database of Religious History (DRH) (https://religiondatabase.org/landing/) was also used for reviewing the presence or absence of high gods.

  3. D-PLACE (https://d-place.org/) was used for cases not found in Murdock (1967).

  4. Although the temporal ordering of moral high gods and complexity is controversial (Beheim et al. 2019; Slingerland et al. 2019; Whitehouse et al. 2019), there is broad agreement that complexity and moral high gods are correlated.

  5. In our first set of models, religious belief variables were predictors and resource stress was an outcome. Models were organized in this way because dependent variables in multilevel models must vary at each level of analysis.

References

  • Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. Hawthorne: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ammār, Ḥ (1954). Growing up in an Egyptian village: Silwa, province of Aswan. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (2002). The effects of ritual. Archiv für Religionspsychologie/Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 24, 167–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, Q. D., & Bourrat, P. (2011). Beliefs about God, the afterlife and morality support the role of supernatural policing in human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, Q. D., & Whitehouse, H. (2011). The cultural morphospace of ritual form: Examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 50–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2011). A cross-national test of the uncertainty hypothesis of religious belief. Cross-Cultural Research, 45(3), 318–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2013). Country religiosity declines as material security increases. Cross-Cultural Research, 47(1), 42–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2015). Why is Mississippi more religious than New Hampshire? Material security and ethnicity as factors. Cross-Cultural Research, 49(3), 315–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48 https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beheim, B., Atkinson, Q., Bulbulia, J., Gervais, W. M., Gray, R., Henrich, J., Monroe, L. M., Muthukrishna, M., Norenzayan, M., Purzycki, A., Shariff, B., Slingerland, A., Spicer, E., R., & Willard, A. (2019). Corrected analyses show that moralizing gods precede complex societies but serious data concerns remain. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jwa2n.

  • Behrendt, S. (2014). lm. beta: Add standardized regression coefficients to lm-Objects. R package version 1.5-1.

  • Bentzen, J. S. (2015). Acts of god? Religiosity and natural disasters across subnational world districts. University of Copenhagen Department of Economics Discussion Paper (15 – 06).

  • Boehm, C. (2008). Purposive social selection and the evolution of human altruism. Cross-Cultural Research, 42, 319–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (2012). Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botero, C. A., Gardner, B., Kirby, K. R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin, M. C., & Gray, R. D. (2014). The ecology of religious beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 16784–16789.

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1533), 3281–3288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C., & Eff, E. A. (2010). The state and the supernatural: Support for prosocial behavior. Structure and Dynamics, 4(1), 1–21.

  • Bulbulia, J. (2004). Religious costs as adaptations that signal altruistic intention. Evolution and Cognition, 10(1), 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E. A. (1980). Egalitarianism among hunters and gatherers. American Anthropologist, 82, 116–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, V. D. (1982). When stars came down to earth: Cosmology of the Skidi Pawnee Indians of North America. Ballena Press Anthropological Papers. Los Altos: Ballena Press. Center for Archaeoastronomy, University of Maryland. https://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=nq18-021.

  • Cohen, A. (1990). A cross-cultural study of the effects of environmental unpredictability on aggression in folktales. American Anthropologist, 92(2), 474–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Divale, W. (2004). Codebook of variables for the standard cross-cultural sample. World Cultures. The Journal of Cross-Cultural and Comparative Research, 14(2), 1–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draper, P. (1978). The learning environment for aggression and anti-social behavior among the !Kung. In A. Montagu (Ed.), Learning non-aggression (pp. 31–53). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Egas, M., & Riedl, A. (2008). The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275(1637), 871–878.

  • Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (1992a). Resource unpredictability, mistrust, and war: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 36, 242–262.

  • Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (1992b). Warfare, aggression, and resource problems: Cross-cultural codes. Behavior Science Research, 26, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ember, C. R., Page, H. Jr., O’Leary, T., & Martin, M. M. (1992). Computerized concordance of cross-cultural samples. New Haven: Human Relations Area Files.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ember, C. R., Skoggard, I., Ringen, E. J., & Farrer, M. (2018). Our better nature: Does resource stress predict beyond-household sharing? Evolution and Human Behavior, 39, 380–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, oracles and magic among the Azande. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Gachter, S. (2002). Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature, 13(1), 1–25.

  • Fortes, M. (1987). Religion, morality and the person: Essays on Tallensi religion. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, C., & Connell, J. (2015). The role of festivals in drought-affected Australian communities. Event Management, 19(4), 445–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, A., & Fessler, D. M. T. 2012. Conflict, sticks and carrots: war increases prosocial punishments and rewards. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 279(1727), 219–223.

  • Goldschmidt, W. R. (2006). The bridge to humanity: How affect hunger trumps the selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadley, W. (2011). The Split-Apply-Combine strategy for data analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 40(1), 1–29. http://www.jstatsoft.org/v40/i01/.

  • Hames, R. (1990). Sharing among the Yanomamö: Part I, the effects of risk. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies. Boulder: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, B. (1987). Alliances and ritual ecstasy: Human responses to resource stress. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 26(2), 81–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P. W. (1988). Causal inference, path analysis and recursive structural equations models. ETS Research Report Series 1988 (1).

  • Jaeggi, A. V., & Gurven, M. (2013). Reciprocity explains food sharing in humans and other primates independent of kin selection and tolerated scrounging: a phylogenetic meta-analysis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280(1768), 20131615. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.1615

  • Johnson, D. (2005). God’s punishment and public goods: A test of the supernatural punishment hypothesis in 186 world cultures. Human Nature, 16, 410–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. (2016). God is watching you: How the fear of god makes us human. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D., & Bering, J. (2006). Hand of God, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490600400119

  • Johnson, K. A., Okun, M. A., & Cohen, A. B. (2015). The mind of the lord: Measuring authoritarian and benevolent god representations. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 7(3), 227–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1990). Risk, foraging, and food sharing among the Ache. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 107–143). Boulder: Westview.

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, W. W., Triandis, L. M., & Wolf, M. (1959). Some correlates of beliefs in the malevolence and benevolence of supernatural beings: A cross-societal study. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58(2), 162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1939). The group and the individual in functional analysis. American Journal of Sociology, 44(6), 938–964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1954). Magic, science and religion. Garden City: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, R. A., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2016). Supernatural punishment, in-group biases, and material insecurity: Experiments and ethnography from Yasawa, Fiji. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 6(1), 34–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P. (1967). Ethnographic atlas: A summary. Ethnology, 6, 109–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1973). Measurement of cultural complexity. Ethnology, 12(4), 379–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P., & White, D. R. (1969). Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology, 8(4), 329–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322(5898), 58–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14001356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peoples, H. C., & Marlowe, F. W. (2012). Subsistence and the evolution of religion. Human Nature, 23(3), 253–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggie, J. J., Pollnac, R. B., & Gersuny, C. (1976). Risk as a basis for taboos among fishermen in southern New England. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 15(3), 257–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, E. A. (2017). Social support networks and religiosity in rural South India. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purzycki, B., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R., Willard, A. K., Xygalatas, D., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature, 530(7590), 327–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purzycki, B. G., & Arakchaa, T. (2013). Ritual behavior and trust in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology, 54(3), 381–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Roes, F. L., & Raymond, M. (2003). Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 126–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roncoli, C., Jost, C., Kirshen, P., Sanon, M., Ingram, K., Woodin, M., Leopold, S., Ouattara, F., Sanfo, B., Sia, C., Yaka, P., & Hoogenboom, G. (2009). From accessing to assessing forecasts: An end-to-end study of participatory climate forecast dissemination in Burkina Faso (West Africa). Climatic Change, 92, 433–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roscoe, J. (1911). The Baganda: An account of their native customs and beliefs. London: Macmillan. https://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=fk07-002.

  • Schloss, J. P., & Murray, M. J. (2011). Evolutionary accounts of belief in supernatural punishment: A critical review. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 1(1), 46–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannonhouse, L. R., Bialo, J. A., Majuta, A. R., Zeligman, M. R., Davis, D. E., McElroy-Heltzel, S. E., Aten, J. D., Davis, E. B., Van Tongeren, D. R., & Hook, J. N. (2019). Conserving resources during chronic disaster: Impacts of religious meaning-focused coping on Botswana drought survivors. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(2), 137–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–809.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming: a meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(1), .27–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shternberg, L. I., Bromwich, L., & Ward, N. (1933). Gilyak, Orochi, Goldi, Negidal, Ainu: Articles and materials. Khabarovsk: Dal’giz. https://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=rx02-001.

  • Sibley, C. G., & Bulbulia, J. (2012). Faith after an earthquake: a longitudinal study of religion and perceived health before and after the 2011 Christchurch New Zealand earthquake. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e49648. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slingerland, E., Monroe, M., Spicer, R., & Muthukrishna, M. (2019). Historians respond to Whitehouse et al. (2019), “Complex societies precede moralizing gods throughout world history.” https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2amjz.

  • Smith, E. A. (1988). Risk and uncertainty in the ‘original affluent society’: Evolutionary ecology of resource-sharing and land tenure. In T. Ingold, D. Riches & J. Woodburn (Eds.), Hunters and gatherers, Volume 1: History, evolution, and social change (pp. 222–251). Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snarey, J. (1996). The natural environment’s impact upon religious ethics: a cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 35(2), 85–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2003). Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 44(5), 713–722.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, M. E. (1978). Burmese supernaturalism. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study of Human Issues. https://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=ap04-032.

  • Spiro, M. E., & d’Andrade, R. G. (1958). A cross-cultural study of some supernatural beliefs. American Anthropologist, 60(3), 456–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, G. E. (1964). The birth of the gods: The origin of primitive beliefs. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern applied statistics with S (Fourth edition). New York: Springer.

  • Watts, J., Greenhill, S. J., Atkinson, Q. D., Currie, T. E., Bulbulia, J., & Gray, R. D. (2015). Broad supernatural punishment but not moralizing high gods precede the evolution of political complexity in Austronesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1804), 20142556.

  • Whitehouse, H., Pieter, F., Savage, P. E., Currie, T. E., Feeney, K. C., Cioni, E., Purcell, R., Ross, R. M., Larson, J., Baines, J., ter Haar, B., Covey, A., & Turchin, P. (2019). Complex societies precede moralizing high gods throughout world history. Nature, 568, 226–229.

  • Willard, A. K. (2018). Religion and prosocial behavior among the Indo-Fijians. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 8(2), 227–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1986). Diet choice, risk, and food sharing in a stochastic environment. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5, 369–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1990). Open field, common pot: Harvest variability and risk avoidance in agricultural and foraging societies. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 67–87). Boulder: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

IS and CC conceived and designed the experiments. IS, CC, and EP collected and coded the data. EP and JCJ analyzed the data. CRE, EP, and JCJ contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools. IS, CRE, EP, and JCJ wrote and revised the paper. This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant Number SMA-1416651 awarded to the Human Relations Area Files for the research project titled “IBSS: Natural Hazards and Cultural Transformations.” We thank the reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ian Skoggard.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

ESM 1

(PDF 188 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Skoggard, I., Ember, C.R., Pitek, E. et al. Resource Stress Predicts Changes in Religious Belief and Increases in Sharing Behavior. Hum Nat 31, 249–271 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09371-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09371-8

Keywords

Navigation