Skip to main content
Log in

Household Composition and Gender Differences in Parental Time Investments

  • Published:
Demography

Abstract

Recent research has documented the relatively poor performance of boys, especially those from single-mother households, on a number of outcomes. Differences in noncognitive skills are often cited as a main contributing factor. However, we still know little about the underlying mechanisms driving differences in noncognitive skills and other outcomes. This article provides empirical evidence that parental time investments, defined as the amount of time that parents spend participating in activities with their child, change differentially by child gender following a transition from a two-parent to single-mother household. Boys experience larger investment reductions following the change in household structure, which may help facilitate previously documented gender gaps in noncognitive skills for those in single-mother households. Boys lose an estimated additional 3.8 hours per week in fathers’ time investments, nearly 30% of average weekly paternal investments across the sample. The difference is increasing with age, concentrated in leisure and entertainment activities, with little to no evidence that mothers increase investments in boys relative to girls after such transitions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data used in this study are from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (https://simba.isr.umich.edu/data/data.aspx#gsc.tab=0).

Notes

  1. Depending on how returns to parental time investments differ between boys and girls, differential returns could either compound or mitigate the effects of differential inputs on outcomes. However, this question is not directly addressed here. Rather, the focus is on testing whether investment levels differ across household types.

  2. For example, Cunha and Heckman (2008) and Cunha et al. (2010) included measures of how often the child goes to musical shows, attends family gatherings, goes to museums, and receives positive encouragement, and how often the mother reads to the child.

  3. Externalizing behavior was assessed through a series of questions about the child’s behavior, including how frequently the child argues, fights, gets angry, acts impulsively, or disrupts activity. The externalizing behavior measures used by Bertrand and Pan (2013) are based on teacher ratings.

  4. The age limits refer to the child’s age during an initial screening. For a small number of cases, the child’s age was outside these limits when the time diary data were recorded.

  5. Exact wording from the codebook entries of the corresponding indicator variables is, Who else was doing the activity with the child? . . . Mother, and Who else was doing the activity with the child? . . . Father.

  6. In the online appendix, Table A2 shows the 11 broad activity categories, along with examples. Figures A1 and A2 show means for the some of the activity categories by gender and household structure. The investment measures used in the main analyses sum across all activities.

  7. Each line in Figs. 1 and 2 is from a local polynomial estimate with degree 0 and using the Epanachenikov kernel. See Hansen (2020:702–706) for a detailed description.

  8. Figures A3 and A4 (online appendix) include all individuals who were in a two-parent household in the first wave and complete at least one survey in Wave 2 or 3 while in a two-parent or single-mother household.

  9. About 3% of CDS observations are from individuals in single-father households, and another 3% are from individuals in households with no biological or adoptive parent present.

  10. In an alternate specification, I include a set of age dummy variables interacted with gender. The results are robust to this specification.

  11. Covariates include controls for other features of the household structure, including the number of biological siblings in the household, a marriage indicator for parents in the same household, and dummy variables for having stepparents in/out of the household. CDS wave dummy variables are also included as controls.

  12. Age refers to the child’s age when the child completed the time diary associated with that observation.

  13. See Table A1 in the online appendix for a comprehensive list of broad activity categories, along with examples of each. Figures A1 and A2 in the online appendix show Wave 1 means for different activity types.

  14. This is distinct from the childcare activity type, which refers to an activity in which the child is providing care to another child.

  15. This is also true for childcare, computer-related, and education and training.

References

  • Autor, D., Figlio, D., Karbownik, K., Roth, J., & Wasserman, M. (2016). School quality and the gender gap in educational achievement, American Economic Review: Papers and Proceedings, 106, 289–295.

  • Baker, M., & Milligan, K. (2013). Boy-girl differences in parental time investments: Evidence from three countries (NBER Working Paper No. 18893). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S., Hubbard, W. H., & Murphy, K. M. (2010). Explaining the worldwide boom in higher education of women. Journal of Human Capital, 4, 203–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M., & Pan, J. (2013). The trouble with boys: Social influences and the gender gap in disruptive behavior. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 5(1), 32–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology, 94(Suppl.), S95–S120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2008). Formulating, identifying and estimating the technology of cognitive and noncognitive skill formation. Journal of Human Resources, 43, 738–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., Heckman, J. J., & Schennach, S. M. (2010). Estimating the technology of cognitive and noncognitive skill formation. Econometrica, 78, 883–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, G. B., & Moretti, E. (2008). The demand for sons. Review of Economic Studies, 75, 1085–1120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deming, D. J. (2017). The growing importance of social skills in the labor market. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 132, 1593–1640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunifon, R. E., Near, C. E., & Ziol-Guest, K. M. (2018). Backup parents, playmates, friends: Grandparents’ time with grandchildren. Journal of Marriage and Family, 80, 752–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Figlio, D., Karbownik, K., Roth, J., & Wasserman, M. (2019). Family disadvantage and the gender gap in behavioral and educational outcomes. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 11(3), 338–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortin, N. M., Oreopoulos, P., & Phipps, S. (2015). Leaving boys behind: Gender disparities in high academic achievement. Journal of Human Resources, 50, 549–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin, C., Katz, L. F., & Kuziemko, I. (2006). The homecoming of American college women: The reversal of the college gender gap. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(4), 133–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, B. E. (2020). Econometrics. Unpublished manuscript. Madison, WI: Department of Economics, University of Wisconsin-Madison. Retrieved from https://www.ssc.wisc.edu/~bhansen/econometrics/Econometrics

  • Heckman, J., Pinto, R., & Savelyev, P. (2013). Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes. American Economic Review, 103, 2052–2086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J., & Mosso, S. (2014). The economics of human development and social mobility (NBER Working Paper No. 19925). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J., Stixrud, J., & Urzua, S. (2006). The effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. Journal of Labor Economics, 24, 411–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofferth, S. L. (2006). Residential father family type and child well-being: Investment versus selection. Demography, 43, 53–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofferth, S. L., & Anderson, K. G. (2003). Are all dads equal? Biology versus marriage as a basis for paternal investment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 213–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, B. A. (2002). Where the boys aren’t: Non-cognitive skills, returns to school and the gender gap in higher education. Economics of Education Review, 21, 589–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalil, A., Ryan, R., & Chor, E. (2014). Time investments in children across family structures. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 654, 150–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristoffersen, J. H., Obel, C., & Smith, N. (2015). Gender differences in behavioral problems and school outcomes. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 115, 75–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, S., Pabilonia, S. W., & Ward-Batts, J. (2007). Time allocation of parents and investments in sons and daughters. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.619.753&rep=rep1&type=pdf

  • Mammen, K. (2011). Father’s time investments in children: Do sons get more? Journal of Population Economics, 24, 839–871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLanahan, S., & Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single parent: What hurts, what helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, C., & McLanahan, S. (2007). Partnership instability and child well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, 1065–1083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, J. (2016). Early childhood behavior problems and the gender gap in educational attainment in the United States. Sociology of Education, 89, 236–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parcel, T. L., & Menaghan, E. G. (1993). Family social capital and children’s behavior problems. Social Psychology Quarterly, 56, 120–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldfogel, J., Craigie, T.-A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2010). Fragile families and child wellbeing. Future of Children, 20(2), 87–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, W. J., Sandberg, J. F., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Hofferth, S. (2001). Children’s time with fathers in intact families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 135–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Todd Elder, Stacy Dickert-Conlin, Scott Imberman, Michelle Maxfield, Kelly Vosters, Keith Teltser, Alex James, and seminar participants at Michigan State University for helpful comments. Any opinions expressed here are those of the author and do not necessarily represent the views of the U.S. Department of Education.

Funding

This research was supported by a Pre-Doctoral Training Grant from the IES, U.S. Department of Education (Award #R305B090011) to Michigan State University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew J. Bibler.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author reports no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 335 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bibler, A.J. Household Composition and Gender Differences in Parental Time Investments. Demography 57, 1415–1435 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-020-00901-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-020-00901-8

Keywords

Navigation