Elsevier

Hormones and Behavior

Volume 125, September 2020, 104823
Hormones and Behavior

Changes in prenatal testosterone and sexual desire in expectant couples

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104823Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Expectant mothers' T is negatively associated with their dyadic sexual desire

  • Declines in expectant fathers' T is associated with decreases in their dyadic sexual desire

  • Prenatal changes in T may facilitate evolutionary tradeoffs between parenting and mating

Abstract

During the transition to parenthood (TTP), both women and men report declines in sexual desire, which are thought to reflect an evolutionarily adaptive focus on parenting over mating. New parents also show changes in testosterone, a steroid hormone implicated in both parenting and mating, suggesting that changes in sexual desire may be associated with changes in testosterone. To test these associations, we followed a sample of heterosexual couples expecting their first child across the prenatal period. We examined prenatal changes in testosterone and two forms of sexual desire (solitary, dyadic). Expectant mothers showed prenatal increases in testosterone, and women's higher testosterone was associated with lower dyadic desire. Expectant fathers showed prenatal decreases in testosterone, and declines in men's testosterone were associated with lower dyadic desire. Testosterone was unrelated to men's or women's solitary desire. Our findings provide support for the idea that prenatal changes in testosterone contribute to an evolutionarily adaptive focus on parenting over mating during the TTP.

Introduction

Expectant parents experience many psychological and physiological changes during the transition to parenthood, including pre- to postpartum declines in sexual desire (Radoš et al., 2015). Perinatal changes in sexuality are thought to reflect an evolutionarily adaptive trade-off between parenting and mating: A diminished sex drive is adaptive insofar as it allows parents to prioritize parental investment, while temporarily putting future reproductive effort on hold (Gray, 2013). Physiologically, expectant parents also show changes in hormones, including testosterone, a steroid hormone associated with aggression and dominance (at higher levels) as well as caregiving and nurturance (at lower levels) (van Anders et al., 2011). Specifically, expectant mothers show large prenatal increases in testosterone, which return to pre-pregnancy levels postpartum (Edelstein et al., 2015; Fleming et al., 1997); expectant fathers show prenatal declines in testosterone, which rebound somewhat within the first postpartum year (Edelstein et al., 2015; Rosenbaum et al., 2018). Changes in testosterone are also thought to be adaptive in that they may facilitate infant protection, increase commitment to the relationship, and/or reduce mating effort in favor of caregiving and nurturant behaviors (Gettler et al., 2011).

Given the widespread belief that testosterone is related to sexual desire (Baumeister et al., 2001; Petersen and Hyde, 2011), and research implicating testosterone in parenting and mating (Barrett et al., 2013; Gettler et al., 2017; Gray et al., 2005), it has been assumed that declines in sexual desire during pregnancy may be due to prenatal changes in testosterone (Regan et al., 2003). In fact, there is some evidence to suggest that changes in testosterone may precede changes in sexual desire: Men who suffer from erectile dysfunction and women who report clinical levels of sexual dysfunction (e.g., due to hypoactive sexual disorder or menopause) show significant improvements in sexual desire after the administration of synthetically produced testosterone (Achilli et al., 2017; Bolour and Braunstein, 2005; Corona et al., 2016; Corona et al., 2014; Isidori et al., 2005). These findings suggest that increases in testosterone may promote sexual desire; however, it is important to note that such evidence comes primarily from studies of people with clinically low levels of sexual desire (or testosterone) and administration of synthetically-produced testosterone. Thus, it is difficult to extrapolate findings from these studies to the general population (Sader et al., 2005; van Anders, 2012; van Anders et al., 2005). Indeed, a growing body of research with healthy men and women provides contrary evidence, showing null or even negative associations between testosterone and sexual desire (Davis et al., 2005; Goldey and van Anders, 2011; Jones et al., 2018; McIntyre et al., 2006; van Anders, 2012). Moreover, to our knowledge, testosterone-sexual desire associations have rarely been tested among expectant women, let alone couples, making it impossible to assess whether such associations can help to explain changes observed in both men and women throughout the transition to parenthood.

Further, more recent research suggests that the relation between testosterone and sexual desire may depend on the type of sexual desire and participants' biological sex. Contemporary theories, based primarily on studies of college-aged and young adult participants, suggest that human sexuality includes both erotic and nurturant components (van Anders et al., 2011): Motivation for solitary erotic experiences, such as masturbation, which are arguably more erotic than nurturant, is thought to be facilitated by higher testosterone. Motivation for dyadic or paired sexual experiences, however, which tend to be more nurturant or intimacy-building, is thought to be facilitated by lower testosterone (van Anders, 2013). Nurturant experiences may be more socially accepted (and more easily achieved than erotic experiences) for women (Goldey et al., 2016); coupled with the fact that women's dyadic sexual desire may involve relational components that are not necessarily related to genital pleasure (van Anders, 2013), associations between testosterone and dyadic sexual desire may be stronger among women (especially heterosexual women) compared to men. For instance, in two cross-sectional assessments of 157 and 177 healthy college-aged men and women, van Anders and colleagues found that people with higher testosterone reported greater desire to engage in solitary sexual activities, but only women with lower testosterone reported greater desire to engage in paired sexual activities (Raisanen et al., 2018; van Anders, 2012). Further, men's testosterone was not related to their dyadic or solitary sexual desire.

Dyadic sexual desire may also be more sensitive to changes that occur during the transition to parenthood compared to solitary sexual desire: Expectant couples report declines in interest for most paired sexual activities (except hugging and kissing), but no changes in interest for solitary sexual activity (e.g., masturbation frequency; von Sydow et al., 2001). Thus, dyadic sexual desire may be more closely tied to changes in testosterone during pregnancy than solitary sexual desire. Moreover, given that pregnant women undergo large prenatal increases in testosterone, whereas expectant fathers undergo prenatal declines, associations between prenatal (dyadic) sexual desire and testosterone may also differ by sex.

Surprisingly, there is little published data on associations between testosterone and sexual desire during pregnancy; longitudinal work that tracks testosterone-sexual desire associations in expectant couples across pregnancy is also critically lacking. An early (and the only) review of studies that assessed either women's testosterone or sexual desire prenatally concluded that women's testosterone increased, while sexual desire decreased, during pregnancy (Regan et al., 2003); however, none of the studies reviewed measured both testosterone and sexual desire, and none assessed these constructs in men. A more recent cross-sectional assessment of 589 healthy pregnant women showed that expectant mothers' lower sexual function (including sexual desire) was not related to their prenatal testosterone (Erol et al., 2007). This study is limited by its cross-sectional design, and exclusive focus on women, which tells us little about how changes in prenatal testosterone might be associated with sexual desire in both couple members. Both the review and empirical study are further limited in that solitary and dyadic forms of sexual desire were not distinguished. An overall null association between testosterone and sexual desire may mask divergent associations between testosterone and solitary versus dyadic sexual desire.

Section snippets

The current study

The goal of the current study was to assess associations between prenatal testosterone and sexual desire in a sample of first-time heterosexual expectant couples. Although Erol et al. (2007) found no association between sexual function and testosterone in a relatively large sample of 589 expectant mothers, they did not differentiate between solitary and dyadic sexual desire, which may be differentially associated with testosterone. Coupled with findings showing that expectant mothers'

Participants

Participants were 58 individuals (29 heterosexual couples) who were part of a larger study of neuroendocrine and psychological changes among first-time expectant parents. Findings regarding testosterone changes in expectant couples have been published elsewhere (see Edelstein et al., 2017; Edelstein et al., 2015; Saxbe et al., 2017 for more details); however, this is the first report to include data on sexual desire. The initial sample consisted of 32 couples, which was dictated by funding

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive information for testosterone and each type of sexual desire is presented in Table 1 by gender and session (i.e., the approximate 8-week increment in which participants were tested). Expectant mothers and fathers showed a pattern of decreasing dyadic sexual desire, while solitary sexual desire appeared to stay relatively unchanged across pregnancy. Across pregnancy, expectant mothers' testosterone increased, while fathers' testosterone decreased. These data are presented for broad

Discussion

The goal of the current study was to examine associations between expectant parents' testosterone and sexual desire during pregnancy. Prior research with clinical samples hints at a positive correlation between testosterone and sexual desire; however, the generalizability of findings from clinical to non-clinical populations has been questioned (van Anders, 2012). Moreover, there are reasons to expect that associations may differ for solitary versus dyadic sexual desire (van Anders and Dunn,

References (76)

  • A.S. Fleming et al.

    Testosterone and prolactin are associated with emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers

    Horm. Behav.

    (2002)
  • L.T. Gettler et al.

    Do testosterone declines during the transition to marriage and fatherhood relate to men’s sexual behavior? Evidence from the Philippines

    Horm. Behav.

    (2013)
  • L.T. Gettler et al.

    The role of testosterone in coordinating male life history strategies: The moderating effects of the androgen receptor CAG repeat polymorphism

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2017)
  • K.L. Goldey et al.

    Sexy thoughts: Effects of sexual cognitions on testosterone, cortisol, and arousal in women

    Horm. Behav.

    (2011)
  • B.C. Jones et al.

    General sexual desire, but not desire for uncommitted sexual relationships, tracks changes in women’s hormonal status

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2018)
  • T. Perini et al.

    Testosterone and relationship quality across the transition to fatherhood

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2012)
  • S. Prasad et al.

    Unstable correspondence between salivary testosterone measured with enzyme immunoassays and tandem mass spectrometry

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2019)
  • D.E. Saxbe et al.

    Fathers’ decline in testosterone and synchrony with partner testosterone during pregnancy predicts greater postpartum relationship investment

    Horm. Behav.

    (2017)
  • A.E. Storey et al.

    Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fathers

    Evol. Hum. Behav.

    (2000)
  • M.M. ter Kuile et al.

    Preliminary evidence that acute and chronic daily psychological stress affect sexual arousal in sexually functional women

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2007)
  • S.M. van Anders

    Beyond masculinity: Testosterone, gender/sex, and human social behavior in a comparative context

    Front. Neuroendocrinol.

    (2013)
  • S.M. van Anders et al.

    Are gonadal steroids linked with orgasm perceptions and sexual assertiveness in women and men?

    Horm. Behav.

    (2009)
  • S.M. van Anders et al.

    The steroid/peptide theory of social bonds: Integrating testosterone and peptide responses for classifying social behavioral contexts

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2011)
  • R. Weidmann et al.

    Concurrent and longitudinal dyadic polynomial regression analyses of Big Five traits and relationship satisfaction: Does similarity matter?

    J. Res. Pers.

    (2017)
  • K.M. Welker et al.

    A comparison of salivary testosterone measurement using immunoassays and tandem mass spectrometry

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2016)
  • C. Achilli et al.

    Efficacy and safety of transdermal testosterone in postmenopausal women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis

    Fertil. Steril.

    (2017)
  • R. Ackerman et al.

    APIMPowerR: An interactive tool for Actor-Partner Interdependence Model power analysis [Computer software]

    (2016)
  • R. Ackerman et al.

    Power analysis for the actor-partner interdependence model

    (2016)
  • E. Bartellas et al.

    Sexuality and sexual activity in pregnancy

    BJOG Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol.

    (2000)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Is there a gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence

    Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev.

    (2001)
  • S.J. Berg et al.

    Changes in testosterone, cortisol, andd estradiol levels in men becoming fathers

    (2001)
  • S. Bolour et al.

    Testosterone therapy in women: A review

    Int. J. Impot. Res.

    (2005)
  • K.S. Button et al.

    Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • C.S. Carter

    Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (1998)
  • A. Clark et al.

    My baby body: A qualitative insight into women’s body-related experiences and mood during pregnancy and the postpartum

    J. Reproductive Infant Psychol.

    (2009)
  • S.R. Davis et al.

    Circulating androgen levels and self-reported sexual function in women

    JAMA

    (2005)
  • R.S. Edelstein et al.

    Prenatal hormones in first-time expectant parents: Longitudinal changes and within-couple correlations

    Am. J. Hum. Biol.

    (2015)
  • R.S. Edelstein et al.

    Prospective and dyadic associations between expectant parents’ prenatal hormone changes and postpartum parenting outcomes

    Dev. Psychobiol.

    (2017)
  • Cited by (6)

    • Testosterone tradeoffs in close relationships

      2022, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      It also is particularly surprising that studies of sexual desire and behavior overwhelmingly focus on individuals as opposed to couples, despite the inherently dyadic nature of most sexual experiences and the centrality of such experiences to romantic relationships. In one of the few dyadic studies to date on this topic, we recently assessed changes in testosterone and sexual desire in a sample of 29 first-time expectant couples (Sim et al., 2020). As described in more detail in the following sections, expectant parents (both mothers and fathers) typically show changes in testosterone throughout the perinatal period (e.g., Berg & Wynne-Edwards, 2002; Edelstein et al., 2015; Storey, Walsh, Quinton, & Wynne-Edwards, 2000).

    • Parental care in male degus (Octodon degus) is flexible and contingent upon female care

      2021, Physiology and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Additionally, testosterone decreased in breeding males after pair formation and throughout female gestation. This trend departs from findings in male Mongolian gerbils [52] but parallels male humans (Homo sapiens) [53]. Subsequently, male testosterone levels did not vary from female pregnancy through lactation.

    • Working memory from pregnancy to postpartum: Do women really change?

      2021, Psychoneuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Future studies replicating this effect could also aim to identify factors that may place some mothers at risk for cognitive declines in the postpartum, such as family structures or sleep schedules. Contrary to our expectations of fathers outperforming mothers, mothers and fathers had comparable mean scores at both time points and showed similar improvements in WM. A possible explanation for this is that although men don’t undergo women’s extreme physical changes and hormonal variations during their partner’s pregnancy, they can experience hormonal fluctuations, such as decreased testosterone (e.g., Sim et al., 2020) and even physical symptoms similar to those of pregnant women (e.g., Couvade symptoms; Mrayan et al., 2019). Future work could address whether having a pregnant partner influences men’s WM by testing couples pre-conception and again during pregnancy, or by adding non-pregnant adults as a third control group.

    This research was supported by grants to Robin Edelstein from the Institute for Research on Women and Gender, University of Michigan and the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues.

    View full text