Skip to main content
Log in

Past perspectives and new opportunities for the explanatory item response model

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Models of word reading that simultaneously take into account item-level and person-level fixed and random effects are broadly known as explanatory item response models (EIRM). Although many variants of the EIRM are available, the field has generally focused on the doubly explanatory model for modeling individual differences on item responses. Moreover, the historical application of the EIRM has been a Rasch version of the model where the item discrimination values are fixed at 1.0 and the random or fixed item effects only pertain to the item difficulties. The statistical literature has advanced to allow for more robust testing of observed or latent outcomes, as well as more flexible parameterizations of the EIRM. The purpose of the present study was to compare four types of Rasch-based EIRMs (i.e., doubly descriptive, person explanatory, item explanatory, doubly explanatory) and more broadly compare Rasch and 2PL EIRM when including person-level and item-level predictors. Results showed that not only was the error variance smaller in the unconditional 2PL EIRM compared to the Rasch EIRM due to including the item discrimination random effect, but that patterns of unique item-level explanatory variables differ between the two approaches. Results are interpreted within the context of what each statistical model affords to the opportunity for describing and explaining differences in word-level performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Wilson, De Boeck, and Carstensen (2008) contends that these are Rasch models by virtue of the following. ηpi = θp − βi can be reformed as an expression of odds, \( \raisebox{1ex}{${\pi}_{\mathrm{pi}}$}\!\left/ \!\raisebox{-1ex}{$1-$}\right.{\pi}_{\mathrm{pi}} \) which is obtained from an exponential form of the first equation as exp(ηpi) = exp(θp − βi). Substituting the exponentiated form into the odds representation gives exp (θp)/exp(βi) which is, itself, the exponential form of the Rasch model. It then follows that πpi = exp(θp − βi)/[1 + exp(θp − βi)] which is the probabilistic formula for the Rasch model.

References

  • Adams, M. J., & Huggins, A. W. F. (1985). The growth of children’s sight vocabulary: A quick test with educational and theoretical implications. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(3), 262–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baghaei, P., & Ravand, H. (2015). A cognitive processing model of reading comprehension in English as a foreign language using the linear logistic test model. Learning and Individual Differences, 43, 100–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., & Justice, L. M. (2011). Analysis of letter name knowledge using Rasch measurement. Journal of Applied Measurement, 12(4), 387–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Box, G. E. P., & Draper, N. R. (1987). Empirical model-building and response surfaces. John Wiley & Sons.

  • Brysbaert, M., & Cortese, M. J. (2011). Do the effects of subjective frequency and age of acquisition survive better word frequency norms? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 545–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bölte, J., & Böhl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Katz, L. A. (2006). Effects of word and morpheme familiarity on reading of derived words. Reading and Writing, 19, 669–693.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaves-Sousa, S., Santos, S., Viana, F. L., Vale, A. P., Cadime, I., Prieto, G., & Ribeiro, I. (2017). Development of a word reading test: Identifying students at-risk for reading problems. Learning and Individual Differences, 56, 159–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, S. J., & Rabe-Hesketh, S. (2011). Alternating imputation posterior estimation of models with crossed random effects. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 55(1), 12–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., Appleton, A. C., & Hosp, M. K. (2004). Exploring the relationship between text-leveling systems and reading accuracy and fluency in second-grade students who are average and poor decoders. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19(3), 176–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (2010). Predictors and subtypes of reading disabilities: Implications for instruction of “late-emergers” (R305A100034). Washington, DC: Institute for Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Ayala, R. J. (2013). The theory and practice of item response theory. Guilford Publications.

  • De Boeck, P., & Wilson, M. (2004). Explanatory item response models. Springer.

  • Ferrand, L., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., New, B., Bonin, P., Méot, A., & Pallier, C. (2011). Comparing word processing times in naming, lexical decision, and progressive demasking: Evidence from Chronolex. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J. P. (2010). Bayesian item response modeling: Theory and applications. Springer Science & Business Media.

  • Freitas, S., Prieto, G., Simões, M. R., Nogueira, J., Santana, I., Martins, C., & Alves, L. (2018). Using the Rasch analysis for the psychometric validation of the Irregular Word Reading Test (TeLPI): A Portuguese test for the assessment of premorbid intelligence. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32(sup1), 60–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(3), 468–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J. K., Compton, D. L., & Kearns, D. M. (2011). Word and person effects on decoding accuracy: A new look at an old question. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 489–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, A. P., Gilbert, J. K., & Cho, S. J. (2013). Morphological contributions to adolescent word reading: An item response approach. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(1), 39–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kearns, D. M. (2015). How elementary-age children read polysyllabic polymorphemic words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(2), 364–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. S., Petscher, Y., Foorman, B. R., & Zhou, C. (2010). The contributions of phonological awareness and letter-name knowledge to letter-sound acquisition—A cross-classified multilevel model approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 313–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. S. G., Petscher, Y., & Park, Y. (2016). Examining word factors and child factors for acquisition of conditional sound-spelling consistencies: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(4), 265–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 978–990.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch, C. E., & Searle, S. R. (2001). Generalized, Linear, and Mixed Models (Wiley series in probability and statistics).

    Google Scholar 

  • Medina-Díaz, M. (1993). Analysis of cognitive structure using the linear logistic test model and quadratic assignment. Applied Psychological Measurement, 17(2), 117–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menton, S., & Hiebert, E. H. (1999). Literature anthologies: The task for first-grade readers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998-2019). Mplus User’s Guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Vehoeve, C. Elbron, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piasta, S. B., Groom, L. J., Khan, K. S., Skibbe, L. E., & Bowles, R. P. (2018). Young children’s narrative skill: Concurrent and predictive associations with emergent literacy and early word reading skills. Reading and Writing, 31(7), 1479–1498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puranik, C. S., Petscher, Y., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Learning to write letters: Examination of student and letter factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 152–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravand, H. (2016). Application of a cognitive diagnostic model to a high-stakes reading comprehension test. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34(8), 782–799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rijmen, F., & Briggs, D. (2004). Multiple person dimensions and latent item predictors. In Explanatory Item Response Models (pp. 247–265). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez, G., & Elo, I. (2003). Intra-class correlation in random-effects models for binary data. The Stata Journal, 3(1), 32–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan, K., & Mislevy, R. J. (1990). Integrating cognitive and psychometric models to measure document literacy. Journal of Educational Measurement, 27(3), 255–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosa, A. V., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2012). Lexical and phonological effects in early word production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 596–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steacy, L. M., Kearns, D. M., Gilbert, J. K., Compton, D. L., Cho, E., Lindstrom, E. R., & Collins, A. A. (2017). Exploring individual differences in irregular word recognition among children with early-emerging and late-emerging word reading difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 51–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout, W. F. (1990). A new item response theory modeling approach with applications to unidimensionality assessment and ability estimation. Psychometrika, 55(2), 293–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M. (1992). The body of the BOSS: Subsyllabic units in the lexical processing of polysyllabic words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1004–1014. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Noortgate, W., De Boeck, P., & Meulders, M. (2003). Cross-classification multilevel logistic models in psychometrics. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 28(4), 369–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1191–1207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., De Boeck, P., & Carstensen, C. H. (2008). Explanatory item response models: A brief introduction. In J. Hartig., E. Klieme, and D. Leutner (Eds). Assessment of competencies in educational contexts, (pg. 91–120). Cambridge: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.

  • Wilson, M., & Moore, S. (2011). Building out a measurement model to incorporate complexities of testing in the language domain. Language Testing, 28(4), 441–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., & Moore, S. (2012). An explanative modeling approach to measurement of reading comprehension. Reaching an understanding: Innovations in how we view reading assessment, 147–168.

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading mastery tests - revised. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery tests - revised/normative update: Examiner’s manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. (2007). Conditional covariance theory and detect for polytomous items. Psychometrika, 72(1), 69–91.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yaacov Petscher.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Petscher, Y., Compton, D.L., Steacy, L. et al. Past perspectives and new opportunities for the explanatory item response model. Ann. of Dyslexia 70, 160–179 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-020-00204-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-020-00204-y

Keywords

Navigation