Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-15T17:11:47.464Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A long view of cumulative technological culture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Michael J. O'Brien
Affiliation:
Department of Communication, History, and Philosophy, Texas A&M University–San Antonio, San Antonio, TX78224mike.obrien@tamusa.edu
R. Alexander Bentley
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN37996. rabentley@utk.edu

Abstract

We agree that the emergence of cumulative technological culture was tied to nonsocial cognitive skills, namely, technical-reasoning skills, which allowed humans to constantly acquire and improve information. Our concern is with a reading of the history of cumulative technological culture that is based largely on modern experiments in simulated settings and less on phenomena crucial to the long-term dynamics of cultural evolution.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atkinson, C., O'Brien, M. J. & Mesoudi, A. (2012) Adult learners in a novel environment use prestige-biased social learning. Evolutionary Psychology 10:519–37.Google Scholar
Bae, C. J., Douka, K. & Petraglia, M. D. (2017) On the origins of modern humans: Asian perspectives. Science (New York, N.Y.) 358. doi: 10.1126/science.aai9067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C. & Laland, K. N. (2003) Social learning in fishes: A review. Fish and Fisheries 4:280–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, C. A., Atkinson, M., Blakey, K. H., Dunstone, J., Kean, D., Mackintosh, G., Renner, E. & Wilks, C. E. H. (2019) Experimental assessment of capacities for cumulative culture: Review and evaluation of methods. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, e1516.Google ScholarPubMed
Clarkson, C., Jacobs, Z., Marwick, B., Fullagar, R., Wallis, L., Smith, M., Roberts, R. G., Hayes, E., Lowe, K., Carah, X., Florin, S. A., McNeil, J., Cox, D., Arnold, L. J., Hua, Q., Huntley, J., Brand, H. E. A., Manne, T., Fairbairn, A., Shulmeister, J., Lyly, L., Salinas, M., Page, M., Connell, K., Park, G., Norman, K., Murphy, T. & Pardoe, C. (2017) Human occupation of northern Australia by 65,000 years ago. Nature 547:306–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. M. & Galef, B. G. Jr., eds. (1996) Social learning and the roots of culture. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. (2013) Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods, and models. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2008) An experimental simulation of the ‘copy-successful-individuals’ cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior 29:350–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2010) The experimental study of cultural innovation. In: Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions from evolutionary anthropology, eds. O'Brien, M. J. & Shennan, S. J., pp. 175–91. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Dall, S. R. X. & Thornton, A. (2016) The evolution of individual and cultural variation in social learning. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 31(3):215–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesoudi, A. & O'Brien, M. J. (2008) The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity 73:328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. & Thornton, A. (2018) What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 285:20180712. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesoudi, A. & Whiten, A. (2008) The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 363:34893501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, M. J. (2019) More on Clovis learning: Individual-level processes aggregate to form population-level patterns. PaleoAmerica 5:157–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, M. J. & Buchanan, B. (2017) Cultural learning and the Clovis colonization of North America. Evolutionary Anthropology 26:270–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schillinger, K., Mesoudi, A. & Lycett, S. J. (2014) Copying error and the cultural evolution of ‘additive’ vs. ‘reductive’ material traditions: An experimental assessment. American Antiquity 79:128–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shennan, S. J. (2008) Evolution in archaeology. Annual Review of Anthropology 37:7591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D., Rogers, M. J., Jaeggi, A. V. & Semaw, S. (2019) Archaeology and the origins of human cumulative culture. A case study from the earliest Oldowan at Gona, Ethiopia. Current Anthropology 60:309–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, S. K., Botting, J., Whiten, A. & van de Waal, E. (2018) Culture and selective social learning in wild and captive primates. In: Evolution of primate social cognition, eds. Di Paolo, L. D., Di Vincenzo, F. & De Petrillo, F., pp. 211–30. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, H. & Rendell, L. (2015) The cultural lives of whales and dolphins. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & van de Waal, E. (2017) Social learning, culture and the ‘socio-cultural brain’ of human and non-human primates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 82:5875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willey, G. R. & Phillips, P. (1958) Method and theory in American archaeology. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar