Introduction

In a predator–prey interaction, the challenge for prey is to avoid being predated, which constitutes the selective pressure for the evolution of defences or antipredator mechanisms (Caro 2005). As presented by Endler (1986), these defences can interrupt the predation event in any of its stages: detection, identification, approach, subjugation, and prey consumption. Defences may serve different functions depending on which stage of the predation event it interferes at. For example, defensive vocalizations can interrupt early this predation event (i.e., alarm calls) or late (i.e., distress calls). This is better understood by the definition given by Magrath et al. (2015a): an individual produces an alarm call when the presence of a predator is detected, whereas a distress call is produced after an individual has had physical contact with a predator through capture or attack.

Defensive vocalization have been primarily studied in birds and mammals (Caro 2005), and data indicate that alarm calls usually act as warning signals to conspecifics (Klump and Shalter 1984; Caro 2005), while distress calls can have different functions: startle the predator, alert secondary predators, and/or warn conspecifics of predation risk (Högstedt 1983; Neudorf and Sealy 2002). As warning signals, both call types may not only elicit antipredator responses in conspecifics, but also in heterospecifics, which may eavesdrop on these calls to reduce their own predation risk at low cost (Magrath et al. 2015a). Eavesdropping can be facilitated considering that the call structure, at least of the distress calls, appears conserved across various taxa (e.g., Högstedt 1983; Russ et al. 2004; Lingle et al. 2012), and thus, different prey species may decode the information about the nearby threat and act accordingly (Aubin 1991; Huang et al. 2018). In the case of alarm calls, evidence indicate that eavesdropping is possible if calls have similar acoustic structure (Fallow et al. 2013; Dutour et al. 2020). Research on eavesdropping has been mostly focused on alarm calls (Caro 2005), such as the study on the burrowing owl (Athene cunicularia) where the individuals display antipredator behaviours when listening the alarm calls of black-tailed prairie dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus), which vocalize to warn kin when danger is spotted (Bryan and Wunder 2014). However, eavesdropping on heterospecific distress calls has been observed, for example, in different bat species that respond to calls with mobbing behaviours (Fenton et al. 1976; Russ et al. 2004).

As presented, most of the known call eavesdroppers are also vocalizing species (birds and mammals; Magrath et al. 2015a). Yet the first case of eavesdropping by a non-vocal species was reported by Vitousek et al. (2007), showing that the marine iguana (Amblyrhynchus cristatus) eavesdrops on the alarm calls of the Galapagos mockingbird (Nesomimus parvulus). Since this report, eavesdropping has been reported in three other non-vocal lizards: two species from Madagascar, Zonosaurus laticaudatus and Oplurus cuvieri cuvieri (Ito and Mori 2010; Ito et al. 2017) that display antipredator behaviours when perceiving mobbing calls (a type of alarm call; Magrath et al. 2015a) of the Madagascar paradise flycatcher (Terpsiphone mutata), which is also eavesdropped by one vocal lizard, Phelsuma kochi (Ito et al. 2013). In addition, Emoia cyanura, from the French Polynesia, displays antipredator behaviours after hearing the alarm calls of the red-vented bulbul, Pycnonotus cafer (Fuong et al. 2014).

Considering that different non-vocal lizards eavesdrop on alarm calls, here we explored whether the non-vocal lizard Liolaemus lemniscatus eavesdrops on the distress calls emitted by a sympatric congeneric species, the Weeping lizard, L. chiliensis. Individuals of this species emit distress calls when they are captured by a predator, and while the lizard struggle to escape, attempting to bite, it vocalizes with the mouth open (Carothers et al. 2001; Labra et al. 2013). Conspecifics respond to these calls with antipredator behaviours, such as long periods of immobility (Hoare and Labra 2013; Labra et al. 2016; Ruiz-Monachesi and Labra 2020). This response should lower the risk of detection from predators, because different predators detect prey by their movements (e.g., Shine and Sun 2003; Nishiumi and Mori 2015). In addition, data from one of the main predator of the Weeping lizard, the snake Philodryas chamissonis, show that snakes reduce significantly their activity when they are exposed to the Weeping lizard distress calls (Hoare and Labra 2013), which can provide opportunities for a vocalizing trapped lizard to escape. Although snakes are almost deaf (Young 2003), they can detect vibrations through the substrate (Young and Morain 2002). The Weeping lizard can be found on the ground, around bushes or perched on shrubs, while L. lemniscatus is mainly a ground-dwelling species (Jaksić and Núñez 1979; Mella 2017), which sometimes can be found on the ground at close distances to the weeping lizard (< 0.5 m; pers. obs.). This proximity may provide opportunities for L. lemniscatus to be exposed to the calls of the Weeping lizard, and decoding the information of these calls can be advantageous for L. lemniscatus since both Liolaemus species share predators (i.e., raptors, snakes; Mella 2017). Since both Liolaemus species display similar responses to scents of a shared snake predator, i.e., reduction of activity (Labra and Niemeyer 2004; Labra and Hoare 2015), it is possible that individuals of L. lemniscatus may also display antipredator behaviours when they hear the distress calls of the Weeping lizard, and in doing so, they reduce their own predation risk.

Materials and methods

We collected 13 individuals of L. lemniscatus [5 non-pregnant ♀, 8♂; mean snout-vent length 45 ± 13 (SE) mm] in the summer of 2015, at Naltahua (− 33.725233 S, − 70.988923 W), Central Chile, after the breeding season (Ibarguengoytia 2008). We transported lizards to the laboratory and housed them individually in plastic enclosures (44.5 × 32 × 25 cm) with hermetic lids partially replaced by a plastic mesh to allow extra climbing surface. The front of the enclosures also had an opening (10 × 5 cm) covered with the plastic mesh. We furnished enclosures with a pot to keep constant water supply, an inverted tile used as shelter and basking place, a wooden stick used as perch, and the floor had a sand layer of 3 cm. Lizards were maintained in an indoor vivarium kept with conditions similar to those recorded in the field during hot summer days, i.e., temperatures ranging between 33 °C and 12 °C and a 13:11 L:D photoperiod. Animals received food (mealworms dusted with vitamins) three times per week and they remained undisturbed at least 1 week prior to the experiments, except for feeding. At the end of the experiments, we returned the lizards to their georeferenciated collecting points.

Experiments

We exposed all individuals to each treatment (stimulus) only once using a counterbalance design (see Hoare and Labra 2013). The three stimuli were: 1—distress call of the L. chiliensis, 2—white noise, and 3—silence. The acoustic stimuli were made with Adobe Audition 3. To control for the variability of the distress calls (e.g., dominant frequency, duration, frequency-modulated pattern), we made one synthetic distress call (e.g., Hoare and Labra 2013; Ruiz-Monachesi and Labra 2020), using the mean values of calls emitted by 13 individual L. chiliensis from the same locality as L. lemniscatus (for more details of how vocalizations were obtained see Labra et al. 2013).

The synthetic call had a downward frequency-modulated pattern, the most commonly recorded in this species (Fig. 1a; Labra et al. 2013), with five harmonics built using the frequencies and intensities measured at the beginning and end of each harmonic. The fundamental frequency was from 2.7 to 2.4 kHz at baseline to the end (Fig. 1b). The call duration was of 180 ms, which was the only parameter for which we did not use the mean, but in this case, the longest call duration with the downward pattern, to ensure that individuals of L. lemniscatus heard the calls. The white noise had the same duration of the call, and it was the acoustic control of the call. For the silence treatment, we prepared a record with a silence of the same duration as the sound stimuli. Based on the intensities measured when calls were recorded, the acoustic stimuli were set to 51 dB RMS SPL, measured at the center of the experimental enclosure. Stimuli were reproduced using a Behringer® loudspeaker placed on the floor, 15 cm in front of the window of the experimental enclosure. We reproduced the stimuli using an iPod Nano A1320 that was connected to a NAD Electronics 3020i amplifier and this, to a loudspeaker.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Spectrograms of distress calls: a natural call emitted by a female of Liolaemus chiliensis (90 mm in snout-vent length). b Synthetic distress call built using the average values of calls emitted by 13 individuals of Liolaemus chiliensis

For each trial, the focal lizard was removed from its enclosure and held in a cloth bag for 10 min to reduce handling related stress (Labra 2011). Thereafter, the bag was carefully opened on the top of the experimental enclosure, similar to the maintenance enclosure but with only clean sand. The focal lizard was allowed to move freely into the enclosure, and once it was on the enclosure floor, we removed the bag and closed the enclosure with a sheet of glass (37 × 30 cm) instead of the plastic lid. Before a new trial, the glass was cleaned with ethanol (95%), the enclosure was washed with soap, and the sand was discarded and replaced by new sand. Using this protocol, we eliminated any chemical traces of the focal individual that may affect the behaviour of the new one. Because variations in body temperature can induce variation in behaviour, we kept the experimental area at 35 °C. Then, at the end of the trial, we took the cloacal temperature of the focal lizard, and if it was not close to the selected mean body temperature of the species (35 ± 2 °C; Labra et al. 2009), the trial was discarded and repeated another day. Thereafter, the focal lizard was placed back in its enclosure and had an intertrial resting period of at least 3 days.

The lizard’s behaviour was filmed for 10 min and 2.72 s with a Panasonic HDC-TM20 camcorder located 60 cm above the experimental enclosure. We began filming after the lizard started the chemical exploration by tongue flicking (Labra 2011). Experiments had three stages (see Ruiz-Monachesi and Labra 2020): (1) pre-stimulus, the first 5 min after starting the recording. (2) Stimulus, the period where the stimulus was presented which lasted 2.72 s. (3) Post-stimulus, the last 5 min after stimulus. The whole stimulus was composed of three identical elements (sounds or silence) separated by two silent periods, which were within the range of previously recorded silence periods. A third silence period was placed at the beginning of the stimulus. From the videotape recordings, we determined two behavioural variables during the pre- and post-stimulus periods: chemical exploration (tongue flick) and total displacement. A third variable was recorded during the post-stimulus period, the latency post-stimulus (for definitions of the variables see Table 1). The short duration of the stimulus (2.72 s) precluded analysing any behaviours during this period.

Table 1 Behaviours recorded from videotapes for lizards. The first behaviour is measured as the number of times and the remainder as the total time that animals exhibited these behaviours

Statistical analysis

We determined the induced behavioural changes in the chemical exploration and total displacement exhibited by lizards due to the stimuli, by computing the pre- to post-stimulus difference (i.e., post-stimulus values minus pre-stimulus values). These differences and the latency post-stimulus were normally distributed, and they were analysed using a one-way general linear model with repeated measures, followed by the a posteriori Fisher LSD tests. The residuals of the three variables appeared homoscedastic and normally distributed.

Results

The acoustic stimuli did not modulate the changes in the chemical exploration, i.e., tongue flicks (Fig. 2a); lizards showed similar changes across all treatments. However, the total displacement time and the latency post-stimulus were modulated by the stimuli; lizards exposed to sounds, distress calls or white noise, exhibited a significant reduction of the total displacement time (Fig. 2b), and they took longer to restore their activity (Fig. 2c), as compared to the silence treatment. There were no differences between the two sound treatments (call vs. noise).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The mean (+/− SE) of the three responses of Liolaemus lemniscatus when exposed to the distress calls of the L. chiliensis, white noise, or silence. The changes in the behavior as consequence of the stimulus (post-stimulus minus pre-stimulus) are shown for: a chemical exploration and b total displacement. The last behavior, c latency post-stimulus is the time since lizards re-start their behavior after the stimulus

Discussion

We explored whether L. lemniscatus responds to the distress calls emitted by the sympatric and congeneric Weeping lizard. Data show that individuals behave similarly when they were exposed to distress calls and white noise, showing no signs of discrimination between these two sound stimuli. This suggests that any “scary” sound may activate antipredator behaviours in L. lemniscatus, and that the distress calls of the Weeping lizard do not contain any special information for L. lemniscatus to eavesdrop on.

Why does not L. lemniscatus eavesdrop on the distress calls of the Weeping lizard? We propose three non-mutually exclusive hypotheses. The first one deals with the call characteristics of the “caller species”. The reported “caller species”, mainly birds, but also mammals (i.e., vocal species; Magrath et al. 2015a), depend heavily on acoustic communication (Marler and Slabbekoorn 2004), and their calls have an active space of at least 80–200 m, depending on the environmental conditions (e.g., Brenowitz 1982; Brown and Schwagmeyer 1984). Therefore, an individual can be as far as 200 m and yet benefits by eavesdropping on calls. Although there is no data on the active space of the distress calls of the Weeping lizard under natural condition, under laboratory conditions the intensity of these calls, measured at 15 cm from the lizard is 51 dB SPL (unp. data), and Carothers et al. (2001) indicated that under these experimental conditions a human can hear calls from ~ 3 m. It is unclear from how far the Weeping lizard can hear these calls, as it is for species such as geckos, a taxon more vocal than lizards (Colafrancesco and Gridi-Papp 2016). For example, the determination of how far the calls of Gekko japonicas are audible is based on human perception (Jono and Inui 2012). Being careful in making inferences on lizard perception from human perception, we suggest that potentially the more reduced active space of the Weeping lizard distress calls, compared to those of the bird and mammal calls, may impose important restrictions to an eavesdropper; it should always be at close distance to a caller individual. In addition, the wider active space of the vocalizations of the “vocal caller species” also dictates that eavesdroppers can normally be exposed to calls more frequently, because they can hear diverse callers from different areas. This can explain why the Madagascar paradise flycatcher, which seems to have a high predation rate (Mizuta 2000) and individuals emit loud alarm calls under predation risk (Mizuta 2002), has at least three lizard species, two of them non-vocal species, that eavesdrop on its calls (Ito and Mori 2010; Ito et al. 2013, 2017). We propose that the call characteristics of the distress calls of the Weeping lizard determine that L. lemniscatus does not have enough exposure to these calls to evolve eavesdropping. Moreover, individuals of the Weeping lizard are spread out between 3 and 10 m (pers. obs.) and under predation risk, they escape toward the inside of the bushes (pers. obs.), while L. lemniscatus escapes toward grassy patches (Jaksić and Núñez 1979). The fact that individuals of both species escape to different areas would lead to a greater distance and less exposure of L. lemniscatus to the distress calls of the Weeping lizard, if for example, one individual was finally trapped inside a bush. Therefore, even though individuals of both species are found occasionally at close proximity, this does not guarantee that L. lemniscatus has had enough exposition to the Weeping lizard calls to evolve as an eavesdropper species, as compared with those lizard eavesdroppers from vocal species (Ito and Mori 2010; Ito et al. 2013, 2017; Fuong et al. 2014).

Learning can be another factor involved in eavesdropping. Some eavesdropping species require a learning process, as demonstrated with the wild superb fairy-wrens, Malurus cyaneus (Magrath et al. 2015b), in which individuals were able to recognize unfamiliar sounds as a form of alarm calling when associated with predator presence. In the experiment conducted by Potvin et al. (2018), there was a selective pressure for the superb fairy-wrens to learn to associate sounds as warning cues and then express antipredator behaviour, despite the novelty of these sounds. However, individuals only learnt how to eavesdrop after a certain amount of exposure, displaying most of the antipredator behaviours after a week of experience. In the case of the white-browned scrubwrens, Sericornis frontalis, adults only displayed antipredatory responses to heterospecific alarm calls if they were exposed to these calls either as fledglings or adults (Haff and Magrath 2013). We ignore the ability of L. lemniscatus to learn the association between distress calls and predation. The only information about learning in Liolaemus comes from L. tenuis, for which data indicate that individuals can learn to recognize a conspecific, but after 20 days without interaction with that individual, the recognition is lost (Trigosso-Venario et al. 2002). However, considering that for other lizard species it was shown that individuals require continuous training to learn (e.g., Day et al. 2003; Noble et al. 2014), selection is unlikely to act on L. lemniscatus without the opportunities to learn to eavesdrop. There are in fact, few empirical studies on the evolutionary pressures for heterospecifics to learn to eavesdrop on distress calls and the rate at which it occurs (Pollock et al. 2017).

Finally, it is also possible that L. lemniscatus does not eavesdrop the distress calls of the Weeping lizard because these calls have evolved relatively recent, and L. lemniscatus has not been exposed enough to evolve eavesdropping on these calls. The molecular phylogeny provided by Esquerré et al. (2019) shows that the Weeping lizard, and so the distress calls, occurred much later than the split of these two species. In addition, the only Liolaemus species for which there are clear evidence of vocalization is in the Weeping lizard (Reyes-Olivares and Labra 2017), indicating that L. lemniscatus has not being historically exposed to distress calls of any other Liolaemus species.

In summary, we found that L. lemniscatus only discerns sounds vs. silence, without differentiating between distress calls and white noise. The discussed hypotheses have in common the relatively low exposition of L. lemniscatus to the distress calls, resulting in a non-existent selective pressure for this species to evolve as an eavesdropper of the distress calls of the Weeping lizard.