Next Article in Journal
Diagnosis and Management of Type 1 Sialidosis: Clinical Insights from Long-Term Care of Four Unrelated Patients
Next Article in Special Issue
Relationships between Dream and Previous Wake Emotions Assessed through the Italian Modified Differential Emotions Scale
Previous Article in Journal
Amygdala: Neuroanatomical and Morphophysiological Features in Terms of Neurological and Neurodegenerative Diseases
Previous Article in Special Issue
Dreaming and Insomnia: Link between Physiological REM Parameters and Mentation Characteristics
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

End-of-Life in Oncologic Patients’ Dream Content

by
Alessandro Cicolin
1,*,
Michele Boffano
2,
Guglielmo Beccuti
3,
Raimondo Piana
2 and
Alessandra Giordano
1
1
Sleep Medicine Center, Department of Neuroscience, University of Torino, 10126 Torino, Italy
2
Orthopaedic Oncologic Surgery, Department Oncological and Reconstructive Orthopedics, University of Torino, 10126 Torino, Italy
3
Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes, and Metabolism, Department of Medical Sciences, AOU Città della Salute e della Scienza, University of Torino, 10126 Torino, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(8), 505; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080505
Submission received: 25 June 2020 / Revised: 27 July 2020 / Accepted: 30 July 2020 / Published: 1 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Dream Recall and the Brain)

Abstract

:
Both non-rapid eye movements and rapid eye movements sleep facilitate the strengthening of newly encoded memory traces, and dream content reflects this process. Numerous studies evaluated the impact of diseases on dream content, with particular reference to cancer, and reported the presence of issues related to death, negative emotions, pain and illness. This study investigates death and illness experiences in 13 consecutive patients with sarcoma compared to paired controls, early after diagnosis, evaluating dream contents, fear of death, mood and anxiety, distress, and severity of disease perception (perceived and communicated). Ten patients and 10 controls completed the study. Dream contents were significantly different between patients and normative data (DreamSat) and patients and controls (higher presence of negative emotions, low familiar settings and characters and no success involving the dreamer). Illness and death were present in 57% of patients’ dreams (0% among controls), but no differences emerged between patients and controls in regard to anxiety and depression, distress and fear of death, even if the severity of illness was correctly perceived. The appearance of emotional elements in dreams and the absence of conscious verbalization of distress and/or depressive or anxious symptoms by patients could be ascribed to the time required for mnestic elaboration (construction/elaboration phase) during sleep.

1. Introduction

Memories are a combination of specific traces encoded at the time of an event, along with knowledge, expectations, beliefs and experiences of such event. According to Frederic Bertlett [1], the memory process is guided by schemata, or general organizing structures, a framework for the organization and understanding of information that aid in encoding and retrieval. Schemata capture the ability to systematically organize a multitude of facts and experiences and to extract the general idea which enables the transfer of pre-existing knowledge to novel stimuli and situations [2].
Sleep processes are necessary for optimal declarative and non-declarative memory consolidation and memory stabilization [3]. Sleep, indeed, facilitates the abstraction of rules (schemata formation), the integration of knowledge into existing schemata (schemata integration) and creativity that requires the disbandment of existing patterns (schemata disintegration). Slow wave sleep (SWS) is supposed to particularly enhance declarative memories, whereas rapid eye movements (REM) sleep preferentially supports procedural and emotional memory aspects. From this perspective, we can imagine dreaming as a “background noise” associated with all of these processes.
Regarding memory consolidation, it should be noted that it is not revealed under all circumstances but is linked to specific psychological conditions. Our unconscious elaboration is not neutral but connected to our emotional experiences. Indeed, emotions have been shown to modulate the sleep-related reorganization of memories, and neuroimaging data provide neurobiological support for an implication of sleep and dreaming in some important functions, such as emotional regulation [4]. REM sleep has a well-known role in the processing of emotional waking-life experiences supporting emotional memory consolidation. This is probably because the neuroanatomical regions implicated in emotional processes during wakefulness are also responsible for the neurophysiological background of REM sleep [5]. Despite the theory that suggests that dreams are epiphenomena of sleep without any natural function, the evolutionary hypothesis of Revonsuo suggests that dreams could support a behavioral advantage by selecting and simulating threatening waking events in order to maintain threat-avoidance skills [6]. Neuroimaging studies on regional brain activity during sleep can explain specific dreams’ features and can help to support this theory, and the study of changes in brain activity and mental content across all sleep-wake states could provide neurobiological support for an implication of sleep and dreaming in important functions such as emotional regulation [4].
Regarding memory processes, on the opposite side of remembering, forgetting may be the result of interference or decay, but it is also possible that the brain “tags” information for preferential processing during sleep, enhancing some memories while allowing others to fade [7]. Recent research suggests that information is tagged based on its importance to the individual, with this information undergoing selective reactivation [8]. Emotional material is relevant to an individual because of its inherent value for future survival. Consistently, ample evidence suggests that the brain promotes memories for both threatening and appetitive stimuli [9,10,11]. Such information is remembered better than neutral information when consolidated across wakefulness, but this effect is further enhanced by sleep [11,12]. There may also be a role for reactivation and reorganization specifically during REM sleep, as many studies have observed an association between emotional memory and the amount of REM sleep obtained [10,12,13]. Translational evidence seems to converge that sleep, and in particular REM sleep, is involved in the consolidation of fear- and safety-relevant information [14]. More specifically, it seems that REM sleep may serve the purpose of emotional valence re-evaluation and adjustment [15].
The link between physical, mental, and emotional conditions, health, and dream content has always been a topic of interest, not only in the academic–scientific environment. One of the most interesting questions concerns the possible role and usefulness of the analysis of dream content in the evaluation of the elaboration and perception of illness, discomfort, psychological and emotional reaction, and of general psycho-physical well-being.
Since 1953, when Aserinsky and Kleitman [16] discovered REM sleep, oneiric activity has been systematically studied in sleep laboratories and sleep research demonstrated that people mostly dream during REM sleep (80%) and, less frequently (6.9%) and with different features, during non-rapid eye movements (NREM) sleep [17,18,19,20,21]. Even if recent studies have shown that REM sleep can occur without dreams and oneiric activity without REM sleep, REM sleep and dreams are frequently associated [22,23].
In the literature, numerous studies evaluated the impact of diseases on dream content, with particular reference to cancer [20,24,25,26,27,28,29]. The above-mentioned studies highlighted that the dream content of patients treated for cancer differed from control subjects, by presenting a greater presence of issues related to death, conflicting situations, temporary denial of the disease, nightmares, negative emotions, medical figures, anatomical images and topics such as pain and illness. These results seem to be consistent with the continuity hypothesis expressed by Domhoff [30], according to which concerns found in dreams are the same as in the dreamer’s daily life [31] and they also agree with Hobson’s theory [32] that considers dream content as transparent, therefore readable through direct access that does not require decoding, thus reflects the dreamer’s vision of the world.
Events and memories from waking life are frequently incorporated into dreams, either as classical day-residues in the following night or after a “dream lag” of about 5–7 days [33,34,35]. Particularly engaging learning experiences have a more robust influence on dream content when compared to more passive experiences, which might lead to underestimations of experience-related dream incorporations [36].
We hypothesized that the complex emotional reaction to oncological diagnosis can be present in the oneiric content through the elements that recall concerns and anxieties related to the outcome of illness and to death.
This study aimed to investigate death and illness experiences in patients affected by sarcoma (rare malignancies of mesodermal origin with reduced prognosis due to their aggressive biology) through a dream content analysis. We also decided to investigate the impact of the diagnosis on mood (levels of stress, anxiety and depression-like symptoms), and patients’ awareness of their clinical condition, intended as the severity of perceived illness during the communication of the diagnosis by physicians, to assess any discrepancies between “perception” and “intention” regarding diagnosis communication.

2. Materials and Methods

For eight months, all consecutive outpatients referred to the Department of Oncological and Reconstructive Orthopedics in the University Hospital “Città della Salute e della Scienza di Torino”, Italy, were evaluated. Inclusion criteria were the following: age between 25 and 70 years, primary sarcoma diagnosis, eligibility for chemotherapy and/or surgery (not palliative care). We also evaluated healthy volunteers paired by gender, age and education. For both patients and controls exclusion criteria were: actual (except for patients) cancer diagnosis, having received previous cancer or other potentially lethal disease diagnoses, being related to oncologic patients, impaired cognitive ability (MMSE > 24), severe or untreated psychopathology, following the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition [37], neurological disorders, dementia, immunologic conditions or diseases, sleep disorders, following the International Classification of Sleep Disorders III edition [38], use of antidepressants, benzodiazepines and hypnotic drugs. The Local Ethical Committee approved the protocol, and patients and volunteers signed the informed consent form. The study was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki [5] following the Good Clinical Practices.
Patient and volunteer evaluation included the following: (a) demographic and clinical data; (b) a sleep and dream diary in which subjects were asked to describe their sleep quality and duration and their dreams over one week, if present (within 7–15 days after diagnosis to potentially detect early signs of the impact of diagnosis); (c) The Collett–Lester fear of death [39], a self-assessment scale that investigates the levels of anxiety and concern about death; (d) the Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale (HADS) [40]; (e) The Distress Thermometer, a single-item tool using a 0 (no distress) to 10 (extreme distress) point Likert scale resembling a thermometer. Patients rated their level of distress over the past week [41,42,43]. Patients were also asked to fill a visual analog scale (VAS) indicating a position along a continuous line between two end-points (0–10 cm), assessing perception of the severity of their illness; in a similar VAS, the doctor assessed the global severity of illness as communicated to the patient. The reported dream contents were coded according to the Hall and Van de Castle coding system [44]. Dreams were blindly scored by a sleep medicine board-certified psychologist; difficult or ambiguous issues were resolved in group discussions. The alphanumeric codes were uploaded into a DreamSAT spreadsheet, (free web version), which provided frequencies for the total series of dream reports and percentage calculations for patients compared to controls [45,46]. Coded dream data were also statistically compared to normative data using Adam Schneider’s DreamSAT [47].
Data were analyzed by IBM SPSS 25.0 for Windows [48] using paired Τ-test to compare the mean score obtained from the Collet–Lester Scale, the HADS and distress thermometer between patients and volunteers, unpaired T-test to compare VAS-communicated and -perceived severity of illness, and the chi-square test (χ2) on the presence/absence of illness in dream content between patients and healthy volunteers. Dream contents were analyzed using DreamSat tools. Results are presented in terms of mean ± SD with significance levels at p < 0.05 (*) and p < 0.01 (**).

3. Results

Out of 21 eligible outpatients, 8 refused to take part in the study, and 3 dropped out (they did not return the sleep diary). Ten patients completed the study (3/7 m/f; mean age: 47.80 ± 14.19 years; education: 11.30 ± 4.47 years; total number of reported dreams: 23; diagnosis: osteosarcoma (6), soft tissue sarcoma (4); suggested therapy: surgery (9), chemotherapy (1); average total sleep time (TST): 404.18 ± 102.06 min; average sleep efficiency (SE): 81.59 ± 17.7% ) paired with 10 healthy volunteers (3/7 m/f; mean age: 45.30 ± 12.85 years; education: 14.70 ± 2.98 years; total number of reported dreams: 15; average TST: 464.7 ± 46.63 min; average SE: 93.10 ± 6.44%). TST and SE are subjective measures obtained from subjects’ sleep and dream diaries. No statistically significant differences emerged in gender, age and education of oncological patients when compared to healthy volunteers.
Regarding self-assessment scales administered to the subjects of both samples, that is, the HADS, the Distress Thermometer and the Collett–Lester scale, no scores were above the cut-off. Furthermore, no statistically significant differences emerged between oncology patients and healthy volunteers in self-assessment scales (see Table 1).

Dream Content Analysis

There were no oneiric reports from male subjects belonging to both groups over seven nights of registration, thus, the relationship between gender and the total number of reported dreams was significant. Only the results of female (7/10) patients and volunteers are reported.
A statistically significant difference has emerged between volunteers and patients regarding the presence of illness in the dream content: among healthy volunteers there were no dreams related to illness and death (patients: 4/7 (57%); controls 0/7 (0%); p = 0.005).
Patients who dreamt of illness reported a greater number of dreams compared to the subjects who did not (dreams including illness: 20; dreams with no illness: 3; p = 0.059).
Table 2 describes the dream recall frequency per night over 1 week of dream recording for both patients and volunteers.
DreamSat categories showed statistically significant differences between patients and normative data, as well as between healthy volunteers and cancer patients.
Table 3 shows DreamSat categories and output.
Table 4 shows significant results regarding the comparison between patients and healthy volunteers. Patients had significantly fewer animal characters, fewer friendly interactions and fewer successes involving the dreamer. No statistically differences resulted in regard to other DreamsSat categories.
In Table 5, the significant data that emerged from the comparison between patients and normative data are reported. The presence of negative emotions was higher in the patient sample, which also had a lower percentage of known characters or animals, less friendly interactions or physical aggressions, a lower presence of familiar settings and no success involving the dreamer.

4. Discussion

The research hypothesis was to investigate the incorporation of the “end-of-life” concept in the emotional experiences of oncologic patients. Our results surprisingly highlighted that the level of anxiety, depression, distress and the fear of death was not above the questionnaires cut-off in both patients and healthy volunteers, and no statistically significant differences emerged between the two groups, even if the severity of diagnosis was correctly perceived by patients. Regarding dream content, we found no dreams related to illness and death among the controls, and a relevantly higher number of dreams was registered among patients who described illness in their dreams. Despite the fact that the sample size reduced the power of statistical analysis, the results almost reached statistical significance. Patients had significantly fewer friendly interactions and successes involving the dreamer when compared to volunteers. Besides, patients showed a higher presence of negative emotions, very low familiar settings and characters and no success involving the dreamer compared to female normative data. Moreover, no dreams were reported by both male patients and controls.
Our results further confirm the continuity hypothesis expressed by Domhoff [30] and data from the literature [49,50], according to which the thoughts and concerns in dream reports are the same as those that appear in the dreamer’s waking life, and with the studies of De Cicco and colleagues and Giordano and colleagues [26,51] that have highlighted, in cancer patients, a greater presence of nightmares, negative images and emotions, experiences and images related to illness, death, hospitalization, and pain. More than half of the women in our sample reported explicit references to their illness, hospital settings, and death. These reports are absent among the dreams of healthy volunteers. Furthermore, patients, when compared with healthy female volunteers, showed a lower presence of friendly interactions and successes involving the dreamer, placing the emphasis on the dislike of self, emerging from the total absence of positive outcomes following a personal and direct effort. The data also showed that patients who reported illness in their dream diary were also those who dreamed the most. The particularly emotional dream content remained in the memory because it strongly interconnected with the traumatic experience when awake, indeed, dream incorporations have been suggested to reflect processes of memory consolidation. The appearance of newly encoded information in our daydreams, thoughts, and dreams may be an observation of predominant importance in understanding the activities of the mind and brain during both wake and sleep [52,53,54].
Regarding the elaboration of the concept of death, it can emerge in a substantially conscious way during clinical interviews through verbalization, can be quantified through specific evaluation scales and can be unconsciously reflected in the oneiric content.
Although subjects were facing a very delicate phase of their diagnostic–therapeutic path, they did not show significant anxiety or depression symptoms, and it could not be due to unawareness of diagnosis since a strong concordance emerged between the severity of disease communicated by doctors to patients and the severity of illness perceived by patients themselves. This phenomenon could rely on the behavioral response of subjects, who experienced trouble in verbalizing their emotions, or it could be expected as an effective early coping strategy. Nevertheless, it could be also speculated that patients found themselves in a sort of “mnestic construction/elaboration phase” required for schemata formation and integration, meaning that health issues were not yet cognitively elaborated due to absent or insufficient mnestic traces.
Given the frequency of depression and anxiety symptoms in oncologic patients [55,56,57], the absence of differences between healthy volunteers and patients for what concerns the level of anxiety, depression and distress is intriguing. Each patient, with their psychological and social characteristics, has a peculiar history and specific tools to manage difficulties. These very unique characteristics can strongly influence the emotional response to traumatic events such as cancer diagnosis, whether in a positive (functional coping mechanisms) or in a negative way (difficult or no adaptation to the disease). It is unlikely that the comparable levels of anxiety and depression, emotional distress, and fear of death between patients and healthy subjects could be due to successful coping strategies implemented by patients in early stages after diagnosis (2 weeks). As previously stated, both REM and NREM sleep are necessary to create the mnesic engram and reorganize emotional memory, which are essential for mental health and well-being. Specifically, emotions seem to have an important modulating effect in the course of memory reorganization, which remains to be further characterized. Sleep modulates the processes responsible for qualitative changes in memory [2]. Until REM and NREM sleep processes (and the associated dream activity) cease to create the mnestic trace, the coping strategies to manage events and related emotions can be impossible or incomplete. As a result, subjects can perfectly understand and be aware of their conditions, but they do not (yet) develop anxiety, depression or death-related fears. The apparent absence of psychological distress in these patients in the first days after diagnosis communication could be ascribed to sleep (REM-NREM)-related cognitive processes and a not-yet completed schemata formation/integration, given that the process may require some nights to be fully completed. If it is supposed that dreams are linked to the evolution of memories and memory processes during sleep are relevant to the managing of emotional responses, then the targeted manipulation of sleep and a memory reorganization might help in managing mental disorders and dysfunctional emotional responses [2].
One of the limitations of this research is certainly the small number of subjects. This was due to multiple causes: the difficulty in finding patients with no other comorbidities and the rare frequency of sarcoma; the exclusion of subjects with previous cancer diseases and those with cognitive impairment or obvious language barriers. Last but not least, many subjects refused to participate in such a critical period of their life.
Moreover, patients were required to send back by mail the sleep and dream diary after 7 nights of recordings. In some cases, patients did not return the diary or it was delivered too late for the analysis purpose. In our opinion, the main reason why subjects did not return the diary was the difficult moment they were experiencing—filling a sleep diary would not have been of high priority. In addition, the research did not consider any re-testing, thus removing the possibility to follow up any changes in the dream content and/or in psychological variables (distress, mood and anxiety). To better understand the timing of possible changes in the emotional response (questionnaires) and in the dream content (dream diary), further studies should include a follow up and re-testing, at least at 3, 6, and 12 months after diagnosis. Furthermore, adding polysomnographic (PSG) measures could help to better understand the neural bases of dreaming and to correlate sleep quality and dream recall rate.
Finally, we obtained no dream recall from males, both patients and volunteers. In the literature, women reported a higher frequency of dream recall than men and dream recall is described to progressively decrease with age in the male population [58,59]. Our data could be due to this phenomenon, amplified by the small sample. Alternatively, this result may not reflect the inability to recall, but rather low compliance to the study or a lesser inclination of the male gender to talk about itself.

5. Conclusions

The appearance of the emotional elements in dreams and the absence of conscious verbalization of depressive or anxious symptoms by patients probably represents the main and novel finding of the study and it could be ascribed to the time required for mnestic elaboration (construction/elaboration phase). Given that elaboration of coping strategies and both early and long-term emotional reactions are influenced by all (emotional and cognitive) experiences associated with an event, it is evident that communication of the diagnosis can deeply modify the illness experience. If a clear diagnosis, a proper treatment project and psychological support are offered while the mnesic trace is being formed, it will not avoid distress but hopefully will reduce the risk of depression [60]. In order to better detect possible changes in the emotional response (e.g., depressive episodes) and in dream content, further studies should include an extended follow up. Moreover, performing PSGs could help to better understand the neural bases of dreaming.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: A.C. and A.G.; methodology: A.C., A.G., M.B., R.P., G.B.; software: A.C.; validation, A.C., R.P.; formal analysis: A.C.; investigation: A.G., M.B.; resources A.C.; data curation: A.C., R.P.; writing—original draft preparation: A.C. and A.G.; writing—review and editing: A.C., A.G., M.B.; visualization, G.B. supervision, A.C.; project administration: A.G.; funding acquisition: none. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bartlett, F.C. Remembering: A study in Experimental and Social Psychology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1932. [Google Scholar]
  2. Landmann, D.-P.N.; Kuhn, M.; Piosczyk, H.; Feige, B.; Baglioni, C.; Spiegelhalder, K.; Frase, L.; Riemann, D.; Sterr, A.; Nissen, C. The reorganisation of memory during sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 2014, 18, 531–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Chambers, A.M. The role of sleep in cognitive processing: Focusing on memory consolidation. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2017, 8, e1433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Desseilles, M.; Dang-Vu, T.T.; Sterpenich, V.; Schwartz, S. Cognitive and emotional processes during dreaming: A neuroimaging view. Conscious Cogn. 2011, 20, 998–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Scarpelli, S.; Bartolacci, C.; D’Atri, A.; Gorgoni, M.; De Gennaro, L. The Functional Role of Dreaming in Emotional Processes. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Revonsuo, A. The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 877–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Stickgold, R.; Walker, M.P. Sleep-dependent memory triage: Evolving generalization through selective processing. Nat. Neurosci. 2013, 16, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Oudiette, D.; Antony, J.W.; Creery, J.D.; Paller, K.A. The role of memory reactivation during wakefulness and sleep in determining which memories endure. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 6672–6678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chambers, A.M.; Payne, J.D. Laugh yourself to sleep: Memory consolidation for humorous information. Exp. Brain Res. 2014, 232, 1415–1427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Nishida, M.; Pearsall, J.; Buckner, R.L.; Walker, M.P. REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb. Cortex. 2009, 19, 1158–1166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Payne, J.D.; Stickgold, R.; Swanberg, K.; Kensinger, E.A. Sleep preferentially enhances memory for emotional components of scenes. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 781–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Payne, J.D.; Chambers, A.M.; Kensinger, E.A. Sleep promotes lasting changes in selective memory for emotional scenes. Fron. Integr. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. McGaugh, J.L. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 27, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Walker, M.P.; van der Helm, E. Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 731–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Genzel, L.; Spoormaker, V.I.; Konrad, B.N.; Dresler, M. The role of rapid eye movement sleep for amygdala-related memory processing. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2015, 122, 110–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Aserinsky, E.; Kleitman, N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science 1953, 118, 273–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Foulkes, D. Symposium: Normal and abnormal REM sleep regulation: Dreaming and REM sleep. J. Sleep Res. 1993, 2, 199–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hobson, J.A. The Dreaming Brain; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kahn, D.; Pace-Schott, E.F.; Hobson, J.A. Consciousness in waking and dreaming: The roles of neuronal oscillation and neuromodulation in determining similarities and differences. Neuroscience 1997, 78, 13–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Nielsen, T.A. A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: “covert” REM sleep as a possible reconciliation of two opposing models. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 851–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Wittmann, L.; Schredl, M. Does the mind sleep? An answer to “What is a dream generator? Sleep Hypn. 2004, 6, 177–178. [Google Scholar]
  22. Bosinelli, M.; Cicogna, P. Sogni: Figli d’un Cervello Ozioso; Bollati Boringhieri: Torino, Italy, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  23. Muzur, A. Toward an integrative theory of sleep and dreaming. J. Theor. Biol. 2005, 233, 103–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sabini, M.; Maffly, V.H. An inner view of illness: The dreams of two cancer patients. J. Anal. Psychol. 1981, 26, 123–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Goelitz, A. Dreaming Their Way into Life: A Group Experience with Oncology Patients. Soc. Work Groups 2002, 24, 53–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. DeCicco, T.; Lyons, T.; Pannier, W.; Wright, C.; Clarke, J. Exploring the Dreams of Women with Breast Cancer: Content and Meaning of Dreams. Int. J. Dream Res. 2010, 3, 104–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Horton, P.C. Detecting cancer in dream content. Bull. Menn. Clin. 1998, 62, 326–333. [Google Scholar]
  28. Welman, M.; Faber, P.A. The dream in terminal illness: A Jungian formulation. J. Anal. Psychol. 1992, 37, 61–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ohaeri, J.U.; Campbell, O.B.; Ilesanmil, A.O.; Ohaeri, B.M. Psychosocial concerns of Nigerian women with breast and cervical cancer. Psychooncology 1998, 7, 494–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Domhoff, G.W. Finding Meaning in Dreams: A Quantitative Approach; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  31. Domhoff, G.W. The Scientific Study of Dreams: Neural Networks, Cognitive Development and Content Analysis; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hobson, J.A.; Hoffman, S.A.; Helfand, R.; Kostner, D. Dream bizarreness and the activation-synthesis hypothesis. Hum. Neurobiol. 1987, 6, 157–164. [Google Scholar]
  33. Blagrove, M.; Henley-Einion, J.; Barnett, A.; Edwards, D.; Heidi Seage, C. A replication of the 5–7 day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to future events as control for baseline matching. Conscious Cogn. 2011, 20, 384–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Nielsen, T.A.; Powell, R.A. The ‘dream-lag’ effect: A 6-day temporal delay in dream content incorporation. Psychiatr. J. Univ. Ott. 1989, 14, 561–565. [Google Scholar]
  35. Nielsen, T.A.; Kuiken, D.; Alain, G.; Stenstrom, P.; Powell, R.A. Immediate and delayed incorporations of events into dreams: Further replication and implications for dream function. J. Sleep Res. 2004, 13, 327–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Wamsley, E.J. Dreaming and offline memory consolidation. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 2014, 14, 433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, TA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  38. Sateia, M.J. International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 3rd ed.; American Academy of Sleep Medicine: Darien, IL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  39. Collett, L.J.; Lester, D. The fear of death and the fear of dying. J. Psychol. 1969, 72, 179–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Costantini, M.; Musso, M.; Viterbori, P.; Bonci, F.; Del Mastro, L.; Garrone, O.; Venturini, M.; Morasso, G. Detecting psychological distress in cancer patients: Validity of the Italian version of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Support Care Cancer 1999, 7, 121–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Donovan, K.A.; Grassi, L.; McGinty, H.L.; Jacobsen, P.B. Validation of the distress thermometer worldwide: State of the science. Psychooncology 2014, 23, 241–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Jacobsen, P.B.; Donovan, K.A.; Trask, P.C.; Fleishman, S.B.; Zabora, J.; Baker, F.; Holland, J.C. Screening for psychologic distress in ambulatory cancer patients. Cancer 2005, 103, 1494–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Donovan, K.A.; Deshields, T.L.; Corbett, C.; Riba, M.B. Update on the Implementation of NCCN Guidelines for Distress Management by NCCN Member Institutions. J. Natl. Compr. Canc. Netw. 2019, 17, 1251–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Domhoff, G.W. The content of dreams: Methodologic and theoretical implications. Princ. Pract. Sleep Med. 2005, 4, 522–534. [Google Scholar]
  45. Domhoff, G.W.; Schneider, A. Much Ado About Very Little: The Small Effect Sizes When Home And Laboratory Collected Dreams Are Compared. Dreaming 1999, 9, 139–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Domhoff, G.W.; Meyer-Gomes, K.; Schred, M. Dreams as the expression of conceptions and concerns: A comparison of German and American college students. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 2006, 25, 269–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. The Quantitative Study of Dreams. Available online: http://www.dreamresearch.net (accessed on 1 April 2015).
  48. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 25.0; IBM Corporation: Armonk, NY, USA, 2017.
  49. Wamsley, E.J.; Perry, K.; Djonlagic, I.; Reaven, L.B.; Stickgold, R. Cognitive replay of visuomotor learning at sleep onset: Temporal dynamics and relationship to task performance. Sleep 2010, 33, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Najam, N.; Mansoor, A.; Kanwal, R.H.; Naz, S. Dream content: Reflections of the emotional and psychological states of earthquake survivors. Dreaming 2006, 16, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Giordano, A.; Francese, V.; Peila, E.; Tribolo, A.; Airoldi, M.; Torta, R.; Mutani, R.; Cicolin, A. Dream content changes in women after mastectomy: An initial study of body imagery after body-disfiguring surgery. Dreaming 2012, 22, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Wamsley, E.J.; Stickgold, R. Memory, Sleep and Dreaming: Experiencing Consolidation. Sleep Med. Clin. 2011, 6, 97–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Cipolli, C.; Fagioli, I.; Mazzetti, M.; Tuozzi, G. Incorporation of presleep stimuli into dream contents: Evidence for a consolidation effect on declarative knowledge during REM sleep? J. Sleep Res. 2004, 13, 317–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. De Gennaro, L.; Marzano, C.; Cipolli, C.; Ferrara, M. How we remember the stuff that dreams are made of: Neurobiological approaches to the brain mechanisms of dream recall. Behav. Brain Res. 2012, 226, 592–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Krähenbühl, A.; Zwahlen, D.; Knuth, A.; Schnyder, U.; Jenewein, J.; Kuhn, C.; Büchi, S. Prävalenz von Angst und Depression bei ambulanten Krebspatienten und deren Angehörigen. Praxis 2007, 96, 973–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Geue, R.M.K.; Göbel, P.; Leuteritz, K.; Nowe, E.; Sender, A.; Stöbel-Richter, Y.; Friedrich, M. Anxiety and depression in young adult German cancer patients: Time course and associated factors. Psychooncology 2019, 28, 2083–2090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Halada, P.; Doničová, V.; Práznovec, I.; Pidrman, V.; Špaček, J. Depression, anxiety in ovarian cancer patient. Deprese, úzkost a karcinom ovaria. Ceska. Gynekol. 2019, 84, 309–317. [Google Scholar]
  58. Brand, S.; Beck, J.; Kalak, N.; Gerber, M.; Kirov, R.; Pühse, U.; Hatzinger, M.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E. Dream recall and its relationship to sleep, perceived stress, and creativity among adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 2011, 49, 525–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Guénolé, F.; Marcaggi, G.; Baleyte, J.M.; Garma, L. Le rêve au cours du vieillissement normal et pathologique [Dreams in normal and pathological aging]. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2010, 8, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Gil, F.; Costa, G.; Hilker, I.; Benito, L. First anxiety, afterwards depression: Psychological distress in cancer patients at diagnosis and after medical treatment. Stress Health 2012, 28, 362–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Distress, fear of death, anxiety and depression in patients and volunteers.
Table 1. Distress, fear of death, anxiety and depression in patients and volunteers.
PatientsControlsp
Distress Thermometer4.65 ± 3.04.70 ± 1.7n.s.
Collet–Lester (fear of death)
 your death18.38 ± 6.020.40 ± 5.4n.s.
 dying27.75 ± 8.526.20 ± 8.0n.s.
 death of others25.88 ± 6.625.80 ± 4.6n.s.
 other people dying27.75 ± 6.329.80 ± 6.4n.s.
HADS
 anxiety9.00 ± 1.28.90 ± 1.4n.s.
 depression12.50 ± 1.812.70 ± 1.8n.s.
n.s. = not significant. No significant differences were also obtained between doctors’ and patients’ visual analog scale (VAS) (4.50 ± 2.01 vs. 4.55 ± 2.19 cm, p = n.s.).
Table 2. Dream recall frequency per night.
Table 2. Dream recall frequency per night.
Night 1Night 2Night 3Night 4Night 5Night 6Night 7Tot N Dreams
Patients254423323
Volunteers333021315
Table 3. DreamSat categories.
Table 3. DreamSat categories.
Characters
Male/Female Percent
Familiarity Percent
Friends Percent
Family Percent
Dead and Imaginary Percent
Animal Percent
Social Interaction Percents
Aggression/Friendliness Percent
Befriender Percent
Aggressor Percent
Physical Aggression Percent
Settings
Indoor Setting Percent
Familiar Setting Percent
Self-Concept Percents
Self-Negativity Percent
Bodily Misfortunes Percent
Negative Emotions Percent
Dreamer-Involved Success Percent
Torso/Anatomy Percent
Dreams with at Least One:
Aggression
Friendliness
Sexuality
Misfortune
Good Fortune
Success
Failure
Striving
Table 4. Oneiric content in female patients vs. healthy volunteers.
Table 4. Oneiric content in female patients vs. healthy volunteers.
Female PatientsFemale Controlsp
Characters
Animals0%6%* 0.033
Social Interactions
Friendly character0%67%** 0.007
Self-Concept
Dreamer-Involved Success0%50%* 0.04
p < 0.05 (*) and p < 0.01 (**).
Table 5. Oneiric content in female patients vs. normative data.
Table 5. Oneiric content in female patients vs. normative data.
Female PatientsNormative Datap
Characters
Familiarity38%58%* 0.028
Animals0%4%* 0.027
Social Interactions
Friendly character0%47%** 0.000
Physical aggression0%34%** 0.003
Setting
Familiar17%79%** 0.001
Self-Concept
Negative emotions100%80%* 0.010
Success involving the dreamer0%42%* 0.016
Dreams with at Least One:
Success0%8%* 0.033
p < 0.05 (*) and p < 0.01 (**).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cicolin, A.; Boffano, M.; Beccuti, G.; Piana, R.; Giordano, A. End-of-Life in Oncologic Patients’ Dream Content. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 505. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080505

AMA Style

Cicolin A, Boffano M, Beccuti G, Piana R, Giordano A. End-of-Life in Oncologic Patients’ Dream Content. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(8):505. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080505

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cicolin, Alessandro, Michele Boffano, Guglielmo Beccuti, Raimondo Piana, and Alessandra Giordano. 2020. "End-of-Life in Oncologic Patients’ Dream Content" Brain Sciences 10, no. 8: 505. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10080505

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop