“I like it” and “I need it”: Relationship between implicit associations, flow, and addictive social media use

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106509Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Implicit associations with social media (SM) are positively linked to SM flow.

  • Implicit associations with SM are positively linked with addictive SM use (SMU).

  • SM flow mediates the link between implicit associations and addictive SMU.

Abstract

The use of online social media (SM) is part of daily life, but may impact subjective well-being negatively, and contribute to the development of addictive tendencies. The present empirical study investigated the mechanisms that might explain this development. An online survey and an implicit association test (IAT) investigated the relationship between implicit associations with SM, SM flow and addictive social media use (SMU) in a sample of 145 users of SM. The results reveal a significant positive association between all three investigated constructs. Moreover, SM flow mediated the relationship between the implicit associations and addictive SMU significantly. Implicit associations with SM might therefore foster immersion into the online world, which contributes to SM flow's positive experience. However, SM flow might also serve as an antecedent of addictive tendencies. The study findings should be considered when assessing individuals at risk of addictive SMU, and when developing (therapeutic) intervention programs to deal with problematic social platform use.

Introduction

In the 21st century, the sharing, liking, and commenting on social media (SM) belong to many people's daily routine (Pew Research Center, 2019; Roth, 2020). People exposed to high levels of stress in their daily life specifically engage in social media use (SMU) to escape their problems and obligations, and to find relief (Brailovskaia, Velten, & Margraf, 2019). Usually, they find what they are searching for on social platforms such as Facebook and Instagram. SMU may contribute to mood improvement, feelings of belonging, and of experiencing social support (Bayer, Ellison, Schoenebeck, Brady, & Falk, 2018; Verduyn, Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides, & Kross, 2017).

However, despite the positive experiences that SMU enables, previous research has shown that intensive online activity can contribute to the development of addictive tendencies. A strong emotional bond with SM can cause a problematic need to stay online permanently (Andreassen, Pallesen, & Griffiths, 2017). Individuals with enhanced levels of addictive tendencies, generally engage in problematic behavior to gain positive experiences and escape negative ones. They often lose control over their behavior, developing a strong pathological need to persist with such behavior despite its potentially negative consequences for their daily life (Griffiths, 1996, 2005). Addictive SMU is a multifaceted, but underresearched phenomenon exhibiting six typical characteristics (Andreassen et al., 2016): salience, tolerance, mood modification, relapse, withdrawal symptoms, and conflict.

Studies that investigated social platforms, such as Facebook (Andreassen et al., 2013; Andreassen, Torsheim, Brunborg, & Pallesen, 2012; Atroszko et al., 2018), Instagram (Kircaburun & Griffiths, 2018), Twitter (Kircaburun, 2016), and Snapchat (Punyanunt-Carter, De La Cruz, & Wrench, 2017), reported addictive tendencies. For example, addictive Facebook use was found to be positively linked to the experience of daily stress, loneliness, depression, and anxiety symptoms (Atroszko et al., 2018; Marino, Gini, Vieno, & Spada, 2018; Ryan, Chester, Reece, & Xenos, 2014; Xie & Karan, 2019). Moreover, two longitudinal studies found that addictive Facebook activity is a positive predictor of insomnia and suicide-related outcomes (ideation and attempts) (Brailovskaia, Rohmann, Bierhoff, Margraf, & Köllner, 2019a; Brailovskaia, Teismann, & Margraf, 2020). Its relationship with life satisfaction and positive mental health is negative (Błachnio, Przepiorka, & Pantic, 2016; Brailovskaia, Teismann, & Margraf, 2020). However, since a tendency toward addictive SMU only affects a subset of the population, this requires a search for this phenomenon's potential antecedents. Identifying the potential antecedents could contribute to identifying individuals at risk of addictive SMU, and protecting their well-being preventively. While previous studies described explicit processes, such as the conscious search for online social support, as an antecedent of addictive tendencies (Brailovskaia et al., 2019a), very little is known about potential implicit antecedents.

It has been argued that the human brain has two separate information processing systems: a rule-based explicit system and a skill- or experienced-based implicit system (Dienes & Perner, 1999). Subconscious associations with objects’ mental representations, for example, that of alcohol (Houben & Wiers, 2008), can automatically steer attitudes and ultimately actual behavior, which explains deviations from rational (healthy) decision making behavior. Previous research found a positive relationship between implicit associations and different forms of addictive tendencies (Stacy & Wiers, 2010), such as addictive substance use, specifically of marijuana (Rooke, Hine, & Thorsteinsson, 2008), but also the addictive use of online media, such as online games (Yen et al., 2011). Based on recent findings (Turel & Serenko, 2020), implicit associations might also serve as an antecedent of addictive SMU.

Implicit associations that include cognitions about rewarding experiences might be spontaneously activated during exposure to relevant cues, fostering automatic engagement in problematic behavior (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009; Serenko & Turel, 2019). Social platforms, such as Facebook, provide cues that might contribute to implicit associations' activation, and foster excessive SMU (Turel & Serenko, 2020). However, implicit associations do not necessarily trigger spontaneous actions that might foster the development of addictive tendencies. Previous research showed a weak or missing link between implicit associations, attitudes, and behavior, such that attitude strength and manifested behavior cannot necessarily be inferred from associations’ closeness (Fiedler, Messner, & Bluemke, 2006).

The question therefore arises how implicit associations contribute to addictive tendencies' development, specifically to addictive SMU. Previous research found that implicit associations are positively linked to the flow experience (Schiepe-Tiska & Engeser, 2012). The flow state is described as “a period during which a highly practiced skill that is represented in the implicit system's knowledge base is implemented without interference from the explicit system” (Dietrich, 2004, p. 746). Consequently, an implicit representation – implicit association – with the relevant behavior or skill seems to be an important requirement for the flow experience. Individuals need some experience with the performed task, which makes a flow experience during a first contact with a new and unfamiliar task or skill less likely.

Flow is defined as an experience of intrinsic reward linked to high levels of enjoyment and pleasure when an individual is fully involved in a specific activity. Activities that contribute to the flow experience are often repeated, even at a great cost (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). A flow experience is also associated positively with the loss of self-awareness during the performed activity (Sheldon, Prentice, & Halusic, 2015), therefore requiring a temporary suppression of the explicit system's meta-conscious and analytic capacities (Dietrich, 2004). This partly explains why the continuation of the activity that evokes flow is not generally reflected consciously (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). It may therefore be hypothesized that implicit associations that specific cues activate on social platforms could evoke the flow experience.

This is in line with previous research that described individuals who tend to engage in intensive SMU as experiencing flow (Kaur, Dhir, Chen, & Rajala, 2016; Khang, Kim, & Kim, 2013; Kwak, Choi, & Lee, 2014). Five typical characteristics define SM flow (Kwak et al., 2014): curiosity (i.e., the experience of curiosity and interest during SMU), enjoyment (i.e., the experience of enjoyment and pleasure during SMU), time-distortion (i.e., losing sense of time during SMU), focused attention (i.e., an intensive focus on the SM activities), and telepresence (i.e., deep immersion into the SM world). SM flow is a positive predictor of self-disclosure, and can contribute to the development of close online interpersonal relationships that foster positive mood (Kwak et al., 2014).

However, similar to the findings of studies investigating problematic gaming behavior (Hull, Williams, & Griffiths, 2013; Khang et al., 2013; Trivedi & Teichert, 2017; Wu, Scott, & Yang, 2013), the flow experienced during Facebook's use was described as an antecedent of addictive Facebook use. The intensity of Facebook use moderated the relationship between flow and addictive tendencies. It was assumed that telepresence – deep immersion into the online world associated with forgetting the offline world, which is one of Facebook flow's main characteristics – might contribute to the development of an addictive bond with the social platform (Brailovskaia, Rohmann, Bierhoff, & Margraf, 2018).

Given this background, it may be hypothesized that experience of SM flow might mediate the previously reported positive relationship between implicit associations and addictive SMU. In light of social platforms' high popularity, particularly among the younger generations (Pew Research Center, 2019; Twenge, Martin, & Spitzberg, 2019), and the potential negative consequences of addictive SMU (e.g., Brailovskaia et al., 2019a; Marino et al., 2018), investigating this hypothesis and contributing to the understanding of the mechanisms that might foster addictive SMU tendencies' development, seem very important. Knowledge of such mechanisms could improve users’ general well-being. It might also provide support when screening individuals at risk of developing addictive tendencies, when developing intervention programs to prevent addictive SMU, and in therapeutic settings when treating patients already showing tendencies of addictive online behavior.

To date, the link between implicit associations and addictive/excessive SMU (Turel & Serenko, 2020), as well as the association between a flow experience and addictive SMU (Brailovskaia, Rohmann, et al., 2018) were investigated separately, and only with regard to Facebook. Based on the presented empirical background and considerations, the current study was the first to simultaneously investigate implicit associations’ relationship with SM, with addictive SMU tendencies, and with SM flow. Consequently, following Turel and Serenko (2020) who showed implicit associations to contribute to excessive Facebook use, implicit associations with SM were assumed to be positively related to addictive SMU (Hypothesis 1). Schiepe-Tiska and Engeser (2012) who focused on offline behavior described a positive relationship between implicit associations and flow experience. To investigate the generalizability of these findings to the online world, implicit associations with SM were assumed to be positively linked to SM flow (Hypothesis 2). Brailovskaia, Rohmann, et al. (2018) who focused on Facebook use and Hull et al. (2013) as well as Wu et al. (2013) who investigated gaming behavior reported online flow to be an antecedent of addictive online activity. Thus, SM flow was assumed to be positively linked to addictive SMU (Hypothesis 3). The previously reported research focused respectively on the link between two of the three variables – implicit associations, flow, addictive tendencies. In the next step, the available findings (e.g., Brailovskaia, Rohmann, et al., 2018; Schiepe-Tiska & Engeser, 2012; Turel & Serenko, 2020) were combined to investigate and to explain the relationship between all three variables at once: SM flow was assumed to (at least partly) mediate the relationship between implicit associations with SM and addictive SMU (Hypothesis 4). That means, the implicit associations with SM foster SM flow, and SM flow enhances tendencies of addictive SMU.

Section snippets

Participants

The investigated sample included 145 SM users (78.6% women; Mage (SDage) = 21.50 (4.99), range: 18 to 49; all university students). Participation invitations displayed at different universities in Germany and posted online on various social platforms from September 2019 to January 2020 were used to recruit participants. The requirement for participation – voluntary and compensated by means of course credits – was a current membership of a social platform. The responsible Ethics Committee

Results

Daily use of social platforms was reported by 88.3% of the participants (n = 128; once a day: 11.7%, n = 17; more than once a day: 76.6%, n = 111), 6.9% (n = 10) used social platforms more than once a week, 1.4% (n = 2) once a week, and 3.4% (n = 5) once or twice a month. Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and the correlations of implicit associations with SM, addictive SMU and SM flow that enable the examination of Hypothesis 1, Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 3. As assumed in Hypothesis

Discussion

The use of social platforms, such as Facebook, Instagram, and Twitter, is part of many people's everyday life (e.g., Roth, 2020). While some people consciously limit their online activity to short visits once or twice a day, other individuals spend hours in the online world daily trying to escape their offline problems (Twenge et al., 2019). The intensive use of and the positive experiences with online platforms might contribute to addictive tendencies' development (Andreassen et al., 2017).

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Julia Brailovskaia: Conceptualization, Methodology, Validation, Resources, Investigation, Data curation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing, Visualization, Supervision, Project administration. Thorsten Teichert: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Resources, Validation, Investigation, Data curation, Writing - review & editing, Supervision, Project administration.

Declaration of competing interest

None.

References (60)

  • P. Kaur et al.

    Flow in context: Development and validation of the flow experience instrument for social networking

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2016)
  • H. Khang et al.

    Self-traits and motivations as antecedents of digital media flow and addiction: The Internet, mobile phones, and video games

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2013)
  • K.T. Kwak et al.

    SNS flow, SNS self-disclosure and post hoc interpersonal relations change: Focused on Korean Facebook user

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2014)
  • C. Marino et al.

    A comprehensive meta-analysis on problematic facebook use

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2018)
  • N.M. Punyanunt-Carter et al.

    Investigating the relationships among college students' satisfaction, addiction, needs, communication apprehension, motives, and uses & gratifications with Snapchat

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2017)
  • S.E. Rooke et al.

    Implicit cognition and substance use: A meta-analysis

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2008)
  • A. Serenko et al.

    A dual-attitude model of system use: The effect of explicit and implicit attitudes

    Information & Management

    (2019)
  • O. Turel et al.

    Cognitive biases and excessive use of social media: The facebook implicit associations test (FIAT)

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2020)
  • J.-Y. Yen et al.

    Cue-induced positive motivational implicit response in young adults with Internet gaming addiction

    Psychiatry Research

    (2011)
  • C.S. Andreassen et al.

    The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study

    Psychology of Addictive Behaviors

    (2016)
  • C.S. Andreassen et al.

    The relationships between behavioral addictions and the five-factor model of personality

    Journal of Behavioral Addictions

    (2013)
  • C.S. Andreassen et al.

    Development of a Facebook addiction scale

    Psychological Reports

    (2012)
  • J. Bayer et al.

    Facebook in context (s): Measuring emotional responses across time and space

    (2018)
  • J. Brailovskaia et al.

    The brave blue world: Facebook flow and facebook addiction disorder (FAD)

    PloS One

    (2018)
  • J. Brailovskaia et al.

    Relationships between addictive facebook use, depressiveness, insomnia, and positive mental health in an inpatient sample: A German longitudinal study

    Journal of Behavioral Addictions

    (2019)
  • J. Brailovskaia et al.

    Positive mental health mediates the relationship between facebook addiction disorder and suicide-related outcomes: A longitudinal approach

    Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking

    (2020)
  • J. Brailovskaia et al.

    Relationship between daily stress, depression symptoms, and Facebook Addiction Disorder in Germany and in the USA

    Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking

    (2019)
  • M. Csikszentmihalyi

    Flow: The psychology of optimal performance

    (1990)
  • Z. Dienes et al.

    A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1999)
  • K. Fiedler et al.

    Unresolved problems with the “I”, the “A”, and the “T”: A logical and psychometric critique of the implicit association test (IAT)

    European Review of Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Implicit cognitions in problematic social network use

      2023, Journal of Behavioral Addictions
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text