Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Open Access June 3, 2020

The “authentic subjective experience” of memory in Alzheimer’s disease

  • Mohamad El Haj EMAIL logo , Ahmed A. Moustafa , Jean Roche , Florence Pasquier , Dimitrios Kapogiannis , Karim Gallouj and Pascal Antoine

Abstract

Most research has mainly focused on the decline of the subjective experience in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). However, few attempts have been made to evaluate whether subjective experience may be maintained in AD. In this narrative review, we attempt to provide a positive view, according to which patients with AD can enjoy, to some extent, subjective experience during memory retrieval. Memory and expression difficulties (e.g., aphasia) limit the ability of patients with AD to describe their memories, resulting in a little specificity of reported memories. However, according to the “authentic subjective experience” view, we propose in this study that the ability to mentally relive these memories could be preserved in the patients. By proposing the authentic subjective experience view, we attempt to provide an alternative view to the general consideration that the patients suffer a diminished subjective experience. This view can contribute to a larger clinical framework that gives a positive meaning to the subjective experience of patients with AD. Furthermore, several clinical and empirical implications can be drawn from the authentic subjective experience view, including the possibility to evaluate behavioral correlates of the subjective experience in AD.

1 Introduction

Memory decline is widely considered as the cognitive hallmark of typical Alzheimer's disease (AD) [1]. Psychological research has been mainly concerned with the diminished ability of the patients to retrieve and relive past experiences [2]. In this study, we argue that patients with AD may still be able to enjoy an authentic subjective experience of memory, that is, they can experience an authentic sense of reliving during memory retrieval. At the clinical level, this view is worth considering because by defending the view that patients with AD possess an “authentic subjective experience” of the past, we attempt to offer a positive view of their potential to introspect into their memory functioning and cognitive performance in general.

This review begins by providing definitions of basic terms (e.g., subjective experience and lack of insight). We then emphasize empirical support to the authentic subjective experience view that we defend in this study. Finally, we discuss the implications of the view, as well as venues for future empirical research. Note that, while this review is narrative, we also performed a literature search for papers dealing with the subjective experience of autobiographical memory in AD. In this literature search, we used MEDLINE (via PubMed) and Google Scholar databases using a combination of the following search terms and key words: Alzheimer’s disease, anosognosia, autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, consciousness, denial, and insight. The search resulted in 464 papers, and we screened these papers to identify those dealing with the subjective experience of autobiographical memory in patients with AD, resulting in the below-discussed papers.

1.1 Basic definitions

The subjective experience of episodic memory can be defined with reference to “subjective experience” as proposed by Tulving [3] who associates episodic recall with a feeling of re-experiencing (i.e., autonoetic consciousness), which refers to the ability to mentally project one's existence into a subjective time. Impairment in mental time travel can be associated with memory distortions in AD. According to the Temporal Consciousness Model [4], patients with confabulations (including those with AD) have a disturbed sense of chronology, such that they are able to retrieve the content of events but not their order of occurrence. Consequently, they misattribute the features of events that occurred at one time to other events that occurred at another time. Regardless of memory, the subjective experience in patients with AD is a complex and non-unitary phenomenon [5,6,7,8]. The same thing can be said for the concept of consciousness of general. Although consciousness can be defined as the awareness of the self and the environment [9], it also includes several phenomenological components (e.g., the subjective experience) and cognitive aspects (e.g., perception, affect, and control).

The literature proposes several terms (e.g., “denial,” “lack of insight,” and “anosognosia”) to describe the decline of the subjective experience in AD. The terms “denial” and “lack of insight” are typically used to describe a lack of introspective ability and metacognitive functioning, whereas the term “anosognosia” is typically used to describe a failure to acknowledge a particular cognitive deficit [10,11]. Theoretical accounts on the mechanism of anosognosia in AD have associated a lack of insight with a disconnection of the “conscious awareness system.” The conscious awareness system is a cognitive system that detects the changes in cognitive functioning based on the inputs from various brain regions [12,13]. Any disconnection between the conscious awareness system and the changes in a given cognitive domain (e.g., memory) results in a lack of awareness within that cognitive domain. A similar suggestion was made by the Cognitive Awareness Model [14,15]. This model is composed of components receiving information from autobiographical memory and comparator components monitoring performances. All these components provide an output that feeds into awareness about the actual performances. According to the Cognitive Awareness Model, patients with unawareness of memory deficits have difficulties to encode those failures into memory, relying on outdated representations of their memory functioning. Research on anosognosia emphasizes the ability of patients with AD to estimate their own memory functioning and introspect into their own subjective experience during recall. While this research explains how the patients estimate and experience their own memory functioning, it runs the risk of denying the patients any level of awareness of their memories (e.g., “lack of awareness” and “anosognosia”). However, the concept of awareness in AD, at least the subjective experience of memory, is not necessarily negative since patients may be able to enjoy an authentic subjective experience of the past. We here discuss the research demonstrating how patients with AD can enjoy an authentic subjective experience of memory.

2 The authentic subjective experience view

In a previous study, we investigated whether patients with AD may report a high subjective experience of autobiographical memories [16]. We asked patients with mild AD and the control participants to retrieve three autobiographical memories. For each memory, the participants were invited to rate its subjective characteristics on items assessing travel in time, remembering, reliving, realness, visual imagery, auditory imagery, emotion, and importance. Besides this subjective evaluation, we analyzed the specificity of the retrieved memories (i.e., whether memories described general events or specific events situated in time and space). AD participants showed lower general autobiographical recall than the controls, and poorer reliving, travel in time, remembering, realness, visual imagery, auditory imagery, language, rehearsal, and spatial detail – a decrease that was especially pronounced for visual imagery. Yet, AD participants showed high rating for emotion and importance. Critically, a discrepancy (i.e., higher levels of subjective reliving than objective recall) was observed in patients with AD but not in the control participants. In other words, despite a compromise in their (objective) autobiographical performance, patients with AD attributed a high value to their subjective experience, especially to emotion and importance of memory. This discrepancy could be due to an overestimation and alternatively to a high consciousness experience during which patients with mild AD possess authentic subjective experiences of the past. This interpretation is further supported by a study demonstrating that despite a compromise in some subjective features of recall (e.g., in the ability to create a visual image of the retrieved event), other subjective features (i.e., emotion and importance of the recall) can be fairly experienced by patients with mild AD [17].

We propose the authentic subjective experience view to explain the discrepancy between subjective reliving and objective recall in patients with AD. This is different from the anosognosia account. While the anosognosia account also deals with the subjective experience of remembering, this account attributes the failure to introspect into one’s own performance to a discrepancy between the expected and actual inputs. Our view emphasizes how patients with AD, and patients with amnesia in general [18], attribute a high value to their sense of reliving as a result of their authentic ability to experience the retrieved event rather than as a result of their failures to experience it.

As illustrated in Figure 1, the authentic subjective experience view is summarized as follows: when asked to retrieve and report autobiographical memories, patients with AD may struggle to describe them, probably due to difficulties in both retrieving and expressing their memories. However, these patients may be able to enjoy an authentic subjective experience of their memories (with corresponding emotions), as this experience does not require linguistic processing. The difficulty to describe specific autobiographical events in AD is in agreement with the large body of research demonstrating a diminished ability of the patients to retrieve and report specific autobiographical memories [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32]. In this body of research, the patients are typically asked to describe verbally the retrieved memories. This verbal description may limit the patients’ ability to detail retrieved memories, which is supported by a research demonstrating significant correlations between autobiographical retrieval and verbal fluency [33]. This suggestion can be also supported by the fact that AD affects several aspects of language, typically starting with semantics before proceeding to syntax and phonology and eventually leads to frank aphasia, a multimodal clinically significant disorder of understanding and producing language [34]. Moreover, these linguistic difficulties may limit the ability of patients with AD to describe memories and their subjective experience. The authentic subjective experience view can be supported by a research on normal aging, demonstrating that narrative style can hinder the ability of healthy older adults to retrieve specific memories [35,36]. The authentic subjective experience view can also be supported by a study demonstrating that narrative abilities influence the ability of patients with amnesia to describe pictures and imagined scenarios [37].

Figure 1 
          The “authentic subjective experience” account.
Figure 1

The “authentic subjective experience” account.

Several theoretical and clinical implications can be drawn from the authentic subjective experience view. At the theoretical level, this view contributes to the understanding of consciousness in patients with AD, especially to the understanding of how these patients relive and have a sense of ownership of memories. The authentic subjective experience view focuses on the ability of the patients to travel back in time to re-experience the past. The authentic subjective experience view can be also considered as an attempt to demonstrate how patients with AD can benefit from an authentic subjective experience when retrieving personal memories.

Another theoretical implication of the authentic subjective experience view is that it emphasizes the heterogeneity of consciousness experience in AD. Research on the subjective experience of memory in AD has typically evaluated different forms of this experience, such as sense of self [38], metamemory [39], anosognosia [15], or even denial [40], suggesting a heterogeneity of the consciousness experience in AD. The authentic subjective experience view extends this research by suggesting how, despite a compromise of anosognosia, other components can be relatively spared in AD (e.g., the subjective experience of autobiographical memory). It is noteworthy that this mixed picture can also be observed for specific consciousness experiences in AD. For instance, individual differences among AD patients in anosognosia can be observed depending on the assessment tool, the stage of dementia, and the type of memory or other cognitive function in question.

Regarding normal aging, the subjective experience during autobiographical retrieval was evaluated by Piolino et al. [41] who invited healthy older adults to retrieve recent and remote autobiographical memories. After memory retrieval, the participants had to provide a “Remember” response, if they were able to recover a specific event with its encoding context, and a “Know” response, if they just knew this event had happened to them but could not recall any contextual detail. Hence, Remember responses referred to autonoetic consciousness, whereas Know responses referred to noetic consciousness. The results demonstrated increased Know responses with age for recent but not for remote memories, supporting the view that autonoetic consciousness is relatively better preserved in healthy older adults with respect to the distant past than with respect to the more recent past. Note that, using the Remember/Know paradigm, a research has demonstrated increased Remember responses in patients with AD [42,43], suggesting a decreased autonoetic consciousness in AD. However, this research has been concerned with memory for simple stimuli (e.g., words and pictures) rather than with autobiographical memory.

One theoretical implication of the authentic subjective experience view is its potential effect on the patients’ sense of ownership of past experiences (i.e., the feeling that these experiences belong to them). This speculation can be compared with that of James [44] who distinguished between the self as an agent (the I) and the self as an object of reflection (the Me). According to James [44], personal ownership, as dependent on the “I”, is accompanied by a subjective experience imbued with a sense of warmth and intimacy. This colorful reliving can be associated with the subjective experience of the past in patients with AD, an assumption supported by our previous research demonstrating that these patients attribute high personal importance to their recall [16,17].

Altogether, the authentic subjective experience view can be considered as the outcome of the interaction between representational content (i.e., the retrieved memory), conscious apprehension, and sense of ownership resulting into the warmth and intimacy that characterizes the subjective experience of the past.

3 Toward a positive view of the consciousness experience in patients

As illustrated in the previous section, one implication of the authentic subjective experience view is that whereas some aspects of the consciousness experience can be affected by AD (e.g., anosognosia), other aspects (e.g., the ability to relive autobiographical memories) can be fairly experienced by patients with AD. At the clinical level, this implication is important, as it emphasizes how patients with AD can generate a subjective experience. Another implication is that by enjoying a fair subjective experience of autobiographical memories, patients with AD may be able to experience a rich sense of self and identity unlike what is generally assumed. This speculation can be supported by a research demonstrating an intimate relationship between the autobiographical performance and the sense of self in AD [33,45].

The authentic subjective experience view is also an attempt to alleviate the social stigma that can be associated with the disease. Needless to say, AD is mainly associated with dysfunction and poor cognitive abilities, and the social stigma tends to depict the disease as the “death sentence” and the patients as “empty shells” who experience a “death of the mind” [46,47]. The authentic subjective experience view can be considered as an alternative that identifies probably partially spared components of consciousness (i.e., the subjective experience of past) in patients with AD. We hope that this positive signal may prompt other researchers and clinicians to investigate, or at least consider, remaining intact cognitive functions in AD. We also hope that this view contributes to care models that give positive meaning to the consciousness experience of the patients and their ability to introspect into their cognitive functioning and, consequently, into their ability to judge, think, plan, and decide.

3.1 Perspectives: behavioral evaluation of the authentic subjective experience

One major challenge for autobiographical retrieval in AD, besides mnemonic difficulties, is aphasia. The degree to which an authentic subjective experience occurs may be partially beyond language-based description. Nevertheless, the validity of this view can be subjected to rigorous research to delineate the behavioral correlates of autobiographical memory in AD using non-linguistic means. This future research may be based on facial expression techniques. Due to advances in the technology allowing a reliable analysis of facial expressions, there is a burgeoning interest in facial expressions that can be activated during autobiographical retrieval [48,49]. A study reported that emotional memories trigger the corresponding basic facial expressions (i.e., high happy facial expressions for happy memories and sad facial expressions for sad memories) [50]. Another study reported higher happy emotional facial expressions during imagining the future than during remembering the past [51]. It would be of interest to evaluate whether emotional facial expressions are triggered by retrieval of autobiographical memories in AD, as such expressions would provide behavioral evidence of the hypothesis that the patients subjectively experience emotional characteristics of their past.

Any behavioral evidence of the hypothesis that the patients possess authentic subjective experiences of their past would be of valuable interest to understand the subjective experience of the patients in the advanced stages of AD. The study supporting the authentic subjective experience hypothesis was conducted in patients with mild AD [16,17]; therefore, research is needed to investigate this experience in patients with advanced AD. This issue is important because advanced AD is characterized by severe language impairment and apathy [52], limiting the ability of the patients to describe memories and their subjective experience. We believe that behavioral studies, such as those on facial expressions, would provide valuable evidence of the hypothesis that patients with advanced AD may possess authentic subjective experiences of their past. Another suggestion for future research would be to examine neural basis of the authentic subjective experience hypothesis. While there is a lack of research on this hypothesis, a research has shown involvement of the hippocampus in the decline of autobiographical memory [53,54]. It would be of interest to investigate whether, despite disruption of the hippocampus, the authentic subjective experience can be supported by other brain regions that are less affected by the disease, at least during the early stages of the AD, such as the visual cortices [55,56].

4 Summary

While research on the consciousness experience in AD has been mainly concerned with the compromise of this experience, the authentic subjective experience view attempts to provide an alternative view according to which patients with AD can enjoy, to some extent, an authentic subjective experience during memory retrieval. Although it is important to acknowledge that some components of consciousness experience are affected by AD (e.g., anosognosia), it is equally important to consider whether patients can positively experience other components such as emotion. By doing this, we hope that this narrative review will contribute to a care model that acknowledges and honors the patients’ subjective experiences, provides positive meaning to them, and uses them as a tool in managing the disease.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the LABEX (excellence laboratory, program investment for the future), DISTALZ (Development of Innovative Strategies for a Transdisciplinary Approach to Alzheimer Disease), and the EU Interreg CASCADE 2 Seas Programme 2014–2020 (co-funded by the European Regional Development Fund). This research was supported in part (for the author DK) by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging, NIH.

  1. Conflicts of interest: Authors state no conflict of interest.

References

[1] McKhann G, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack Jr. CR, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement: J Alzheimer's Assoc. 2011;7(3):263–9.10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[2] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL, Kapogiannis D. Autobiographical memory decline in Alzheimer's disease, a theoretical and clinical overview. Ageing Res Rev. 2015;23(Pt B):183–92.10.1016/j.arr.2015.07.001Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[3] Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:1–25.10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[4] Dalla Barba G, Cappelletti JY, Signorini M, Denes G. Confabulation: remembering ‘another’ past, planning ‘another’ future. Neurocase. 1997;3(6):425–36.Search in Google Scholar

[5] Vasterling JJ, Seltzer B, Carpenter BD, Thompson KA. Unawareness of social interaction and emotional control deficits in alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 1997;4(4):280–9.10.1080/13825589708256653Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[6] Starkstein SE, Sabe L, Chemerinski E, Jason L, Leiguarda R. Two domains of anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat. 1996;61(5):485–90.10.1136/jnnp.61.5.485Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[7] Avondino E, Antoine P. Heterogeneity of cognitive anosognosia and its variation with the severity of dementia in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2015;50(1):89–99.10.3233/JAD-150496Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[8] Antoine P, Nandrino JL, Billiet C. Awareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease patients: analysis of performance prediction discrepancies. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;67(4):237–44.10.1111/pcn.12050Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[9] Jennett B, Plum F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search of a name. Lancet. 1972;1(7753):734–7.10.1016/S0140-6736(72)90242-5Search in Google Scholar

[10] Amanzio M, Torta DM, Sacco K, Cauda F, D'Agata F, Duca S, et al. Unawareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease: role of the cingulate cortex. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 4):1061–76.10.1093/brain/awr020Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[11] Babinski J. Contribution a l’etude des troubles mentaux dans l’hémiplégie organique cérébrale (anosognosie). Revue Neurologique. 1914;27:845–7.Search in Google Scholar

[12] Grut M, Jorm AF, Fratiglioni L, Forsell Y, Viitanen M, Winblad B. Memory complaints of elderly people in a population survey: variation according to dementia stage and depression. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1993;41(12):1295–300.10.1111/j.1532-5415.1993.tb06478.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[13] McGlynn SM, Schacter DL. Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological syndromes. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1989;11(2):143–205.10.1080/01688638908400882Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[14] Agnew SK, Morris RG. The heterogeneity of anosognosia for memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: a review of the literature and a proposed model. Aging Mental Health. 1998;2(1):7–19.10.1080/13607869856876Search in Google Scholar

[15] Morris RG, Mograbi DC. Anosognosia, autobiographical memory and self knowledge in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 2013;49(6):1553–65.10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.006Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[16] El Haj M, Antoine P. Discrepancy between subjective autobiographical reliving and objective recall: the past as seen by Alzheimer’s disease patients. Consciousness Cognit. 2017;49:110–6.10.1016/j.concog.2017.01.009Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[17] El Haj M, Kapogiannis D, Antoine P. Phenomenological reliving and visual imagery during autobiographical recall in Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2016;52(2):421–31.10.3233/JAD-151122Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[18] El Haj M, Nandrino JL. Seeing life through rose-colored spectacles: autobiographical memory as experienced in Korsakoff’s syndrome. Consciousness Cognit. 2018;60:9–16.10.1016/j.concog.2018.02.007Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[19] Barnabe A, Whitehead V, Pilon R, Arsenault-Lapierre G, Chertkow H. Autobiographical memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a comparison between the Levine and Kopelman interview methodologies. Hippocampus. 2012;22(9):1809–25.10.1002/hipo.22015Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[20] Graham KS, Hodges JR. Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex in long-term memory storage: evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 1997;11(1):77–89.10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.77Search in Google Scholar

[21] Greene JD, Hodges JR, Baddeley AD. Autobiographical memory and executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33(12):1647–70.10.1016/0028-3932(95)00046-1Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[22] Irish M, Lawlor BA, O'Mara SM, Coen RF. Impaired capacity for autonoetic reliving during autobiographical event recall in mild Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 2011;47(2):236–49.10.1016/j.cortex.2010.01.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[23] Ivanoiu A, Cooper JM, Shanks MF, Venneri A. Patterns of impairment in autobiographical memory in the degenerative dementias constrain models of memory. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(10):1936–55.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.030Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[24] Leyhe T, Muller S, Milian M, Eschweiler GW, Saur R. Impairment of episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in patients with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(12):2464–9.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.018Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[25] Muller S, Saur R, Greve B, Melms A, Hautzinger M, Fallgatter AJ, et al. Similar autobiographical memory impairment in long-term secondary progressive multiple sclerosis and Alzheimer's disease. Mult Scler. 2013;19(2):225–32.10.1177/1352458512450352Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[26] Seidl U, Lueken U, Thomann PA, Geider J, Schroder J. Autobiographical memory deficits in Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2011;27(3):567–74.10.3233/JAD-2011-110014Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[27] El Haj M, Postal V, Le Gall D, Allain P. Directed forgetting of autobiographical memory in mild Alzheimer's disease. Memory. 2011;19(8):993–1003.10.1080/09658211.2011.626428Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[28] El Haj M, Fasotti L, Allain P. The involuntary nature of music-evoked autobiographical memories in Alzheimer's disease. Conscious Cogn. 2012;21(1):238–46.10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.005Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[29] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL, Gely-Nargeot MC, Raffard S. Self-defining memories during exposure to music in Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27(10):1719–30.10.1017/S1041610215000812Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[30] De Simone MS, Fadda L, Perri R, Aloisi M, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Does retrieval frequency account for the pattern of autobiographical memory loss in early Alzheimer's disease patients? Neuropsychologia. 2016;80:194–200.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.024Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[31] Martinelli P, Anssens A, Sperduti M, Piolino P. The influence of normal aging and Alzheimer's disease in autobiographical memory highly related to the self. Neuropsychology. 2013;27(1):69–78.10.1037/a0030453Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[32] Kirk M, Berntsen D. A short cut to the past: cueing via concrete objects improves autobiographical memory retrieval in Alzheimer's disease patients. Neuropsychologia. 2018;110:113–22.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.034Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[33] El Haj M, Antoine P. Describe yourself to improve your autobiographical memory: a study in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 2017;88:165–72.10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.004Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[34] Kirshner HS. Primary progressive aphasia and Alzheimer's disease: brief history, recent evidence. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2012;12(6):709–14.10.1007/s11910-012-0307-2Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[35] Gaesser B, Sacchetti DC, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Characterizing age-related changes in remembering the past and imagining the future. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(1):80–4.10.1037/a0021054Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[36] Madore KP, Schacter DL. An episodic specificity induction enhances means-end problem solving in young and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2014;29(4):913–24.10.1037/a0038209Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[37] Dewar M, Neroni MA, Zeman A, Beschin N, Della Sala S. Visual complexity accentuates picture-description deficit in amnesia. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):689–96.10.1037/neu0000310Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[38] Prebble SC, Addis DR, Tippett LJ. Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(4):815–40.10.1037/a0030146Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[39] Souchay C. Metamemory in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 2007;43(7):987–1003.10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70696-8Search in Google Scholar

[40] Sevush S, Leve N. Denial of memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatr. 1993;150(5):748–51.10.1037/e500382006-009Search in Google Scholar

[41] Piolino P, Desgranges B, Clarys D, Guillery-Girard B, Taconnat L, Isingrini M, et al. Autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective in aging. Psychol Aging. 2006;21(3):510–25.10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.510Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[42] Hudon C, Belleville S, Gauthier S. The assessment of recognition memory using the Remember/Know procedure in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and probable Alzheimer's disease. Brain Cogn. 2009;70(1):171–9.10.1016/j.bandc.2009.01.009Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[43] Ally BA. Using pictures and words to understand recognition memory deterioration in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a review. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2012;12(6):687–94.10.1007/s11910-012-0310-7Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[44] James W. Psychology: the briefer course. New York, NY: Holt; 1892.10.1037/11060-000Search in Google Scholar

[45] Addis DR, Tippett LJ. Memory of myself: autobiographical memory and identity in Alzheimer's disease. Memory. 2004;12(1):56–74.10.1080/09658210244000423Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[46] Van Gorp B, Vercruysse T. Frames and counter-frames giving meaning to dementia: a framing analysis of media content. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(8):1274–81.10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.12.045Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[47] Beard RL, Neary TM. Making sense of nonsense: experiences of mild cognitive impairment. Sociol Health Illn. 2013;35(1):130–46.10.1111/j.1467-9566.2012.01481.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[48] El Haj M, Daoudi M, Gallouj K, Moustafa AA, Nandrino J-L. When your face describes your memories: facial expressions during retrieval of autobiographical memories. Rev Neurosci. 2018;29(8):861–72.10.1515/revneuro-2018-0001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[49] Gandolphe MC, Nandrino JL, Delelis G, Ducro C, Lavallee A, Saloppe X, et al. Positive facial expressions during retrieval of self-defining memories. J Integr Neurosci. 2018;17(3–4):367–76.10.3233/JIN-170073Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[50] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL. More emotional facial expressions during episodic than during semantic autobiographical retrieval. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci. 2016;16(2):374–81.10.3758/s13415-015-0397-9Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[51] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL. Facial expressions triggered by imagining the future. J Integr Neurosci. 2017;16(4):483–92.10.3233/JIN-170030Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[52] Klimova B, Maresova P, Valis M, Hort J, Kuca K. Alzheimer's disease and language impairments: social intervention and medical treatment. Clin Interv Aging. 2015;10:1401–7.10.2147/CIA.S89714Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[53] Philippi N, Noblet V, Botzung A, Despres O, Renard F, Sfikas G, et al. MRI-based volumetry correlates of autobiographical memory in Alzheimer's disease. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e46200.10.1371/journal.pone.0046200Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[54] Philippi N, Botzung A, Noblet V, Rousseau F, Despres O, Cretin B, et al. Impaired emotional autobiographical memory associated with right amygdalar-hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer's disease patients. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:21.10.3389/fnagi.2015.00021Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[55] Markowitsch HJ, Vandekerckhove MM, Lanfermann H, Russ MO. Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and happy autobiographical memories. Cortex. 2003;39(4–5):643–65.10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70858-XSearch in Google Scholar

[56] Piefke M, Weiss PH, Zilles K, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR. Differential remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiographical memory. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 3):650–68.10.1093/brain/awg064Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2020-03-18
Revised: 2020-04-23
Accepted: 2020-05-04
Published Online: 2020-06-03

© 2020 Mohamad El Haj et al., published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 25.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/tnsci-2020-0123/html
Scroll to top button