Introduction

General animal behavior is often described by activity patterns, which can be considered as an integration of several behaviors including movement, foraging, and social interactions. The activity patterns are influenced both by internal (i.e., sex, age, and reproductive status) and external factors (i.e., food availability, human presence, and conspecific interactions) (Leuchtenberger et al. 2014). Behavioral patterns in large carnivores are well documented worldwide, especially for brown bears (Ursus arctos). Radio telemetry and direct observation are the most commonly used methods in Europe and North America. However, monitoring becomes more complicated in low-density populations of elusive animals (Treves and Karanth 2003; Ordiz et al. 2013; Alibhai et al. 2017; Zarzo-Arias et al. 2018).

Many studies found that wild brown bears exhibit a wide variety of activity patterns that influence their survival or reproductive output (Fortin et al. 2016; Hertel et al. 2016; Frank et al. 2017). While in North America, brown bears are largely diurnal (Klinka and Reimchen 2002; Munro et al. 2007); European populations predominantly exhibit nocturnal and crepuscular patterns (Olson et al. 1998; Kaczensky et al. 2006; Ordiz et al. 2014; but see Zarzo-Arias et al. 2018). The type and the intensity of disturbances vary seasonally. Consequently, this variation may affect the behavioral responses of individuals (Ordiz et al. 2016). In Europe, human presence and activities and habitat loss have become the principal factors affecting this species, restricting most of the bear distribution to forested and mountainous areas (Martin et al. 2010; Piédallu et al. 2017). However, also these natural areas are affected by high human disturbance, and animals shift their activity away from periods of increased human presence during the daylight (Brook et al. 2012). For example, Scandinavian bears decrease daytime activity in response to encounters with humans in areas of higher road density (Ordiz et al. 2016). Avoidance behavior in human-dominated areas is important for human-wildlife coexistence. Nevertheless, the presence of non-elusive individuals, possibly adapted to humans, increases the risk of persecution and conflicts that might affect the survival of the species. For instance, habituated individuals visiting populated areas are often considered as “conflict bears” and removed from population to reduce risk situations between humans and bears (Swenson 1999; Wiegand et al. 2004). Apart from human pressures, intraspecific predation during the mating season or reproductive competition may influence responses in the behavior of individuals of certain age and sex (Steyaert et al. 2013). In fact, females with offspring and subadults may reduce the risk of predation or conspecific competition by spatiotemporal segregation strategies (Hrdy 1979; Steyaert et al. 2013). Offspring predation in the mating season has been widely described in mammals, including brown bears (Ebensperger 1998; Packer 2001; Dahle and Swenson 2003; McLellan 2005). Many studies showed that females with offspring shift from nocturnal to diurnal activity to decrease the chances of male encounters and the risk of infanticide (Stone et al. 2006; Steyaert et al. 2013). Subadults show similar wariness behavior, often explained by the avoidance of adult and more aggressive or competitive individuals (Kaczensky et al. 2006). Further, behavioral responses are adaptive to the type and intensity of the disturbance. With increasing intensity of the intraspecific disturbance, vulnerable bears become more diurnal, increasing the exposition to humans (Kaczensky et al. 2006; Ordiz et al. 2014) and reducing the avoidance response to human presence (Zarzo-Arias et al. 2018).

Understanding large carnivores’ behavior variation should help carnivore conservation, especially in human-dominated landscapes (Ordiz et al. 2016; Zarzo-Arias et al. 2018). Activity patterns of reintroduced groups are still poorly known, like the Pyrenean bear population. This population is isolated and considered as one of the most endangered in Europe (Chapron et al. 2009; Martin et al. 2012), with a minimum of 46 individuals detected in 2017 (see also Sentilles et al. 2018 “Suivi de l’ours brun dans les Pyrénées françaises”, Annual report 2018 of ONCFS). Human persecution during the last century almost exterminated the native population. To ensure the survival of the population, 11 translocations from Slovenia have been done since 1996. Under growing population density, the increase of recreational activities in the mountain range, particularly in summer and autumn, may increase chances of human-bear encounters in the area (Sahlén 2013; Fortin et al. 2016; Penteriani et al. 2016). Further, negative social perception, linked to the rising number of attacks on free-ranging livestock and risk of attack on humans, may affect conservational efforts to protect this species in the Pyrenean area (Piédallu et al. 2016; Bombieri et al. 2018). Thus, a better understanding of the temporal patterns of this brown bear population in such a human-modified landscape with high levels of human encroachment should help its conservation and mitigate conflicts (Woodroffe et al. 2005).

As a part of a long term project (2010–2017) of bear monitoring in the Pyrenean Mountains of Spain and France using camera trap surveys, we described, for the first time in Europe, the activity pattern of a reintroduced brown bear population. Our main objective was to assess if activity patterns depending on a given type of disturbance experienced by bears differ between sex and age classes. We expected that the most vulnerable bear class, females with offspring and subadults, segregate in time from those dominant or competitive individuals, as an avoidance strategy during seasons with higher intraspecific disturbance. Furthermore, we expected that exposition to human disturbance might increase the tolerance of bears for humans, boosting the avoidance of nocturnal bears.

Materials and methods

Study area

The study was carried out along the brown bear distribution range in Central Pyrenees, between France and Spain (from 44° 12′ N 7° 18′ W to 41° 0′ N 3° 48′ E, Fig. 1). The 7400-km2 study area covers several protected areas of Natura 2000 network and natural parks. Temperatures oscillate between 20 °C in summer and − 10 °C in winter. The landscape is characterized by large massifs and valleys with relatively steep slopes, in a range between 1.300 and 2.500 m. More than 40% of the area is forested, dominated by beech (Fagus sylvatica), silver fir (Abies alba), mixed beech-fir forests, other coniferous forests (Pinus uncinata, Pinus sylvestris, and Picea abies), and deciduous forests composed by oaks (Quercus robur and Quercus pubescens), chestnut (Castanea sativa), hazels (Corylus avellana), and wild cherry (Prunus avium). Further, brown bear distribution extends to alpine, subalpine, and montane pastures and scrubs (Martin et al. 2012). Human density in this mountainous area has increased for the last decades, encouraging the development of the farming and beekeeping sectors, and different recreational activities, especially during summer and autumn, including sports and hunting. Free-ranging livestock occupies the montane meadows of the Pyrenean landscape during summer and autumn, between 1.500 and 2.500 m, while beehives are located in the lower meadows.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Distribution of the brown bear population in the Pyrenees (France and Spain)

Data collection

Since 2010 till 2017, we used camera-trapping methods to conduct bear activity monitoring during three seasons: spring (March 21st–Jun 20th), summer (Jun 21st–Sep 20th), and autumn (Sep 21st–Dec 20th). We defined two disturbance types according to the season: (1) intraspecific disturbance in spring, mainly through competition between dominant and vulnerable individuals during the mating season; and (2) human disturbance in summer and autumn, from touristic activities, extensive farming, and hunting within the bear distribution range. The monitoring was not continuous. We deployed an average of 48 camera traps (SE = 15) per year throughout the brown bear distribution area. Most of the cameras were active for 275 days/year; however, due to technical issues, the number of camera traps and the trapping days varied over the sampling period, ranging between 34 and 275 trapping days per year. We distributed the camera traps mainly in forested areas, attaching them on trees approximately 0.5–1.0 m above the ground. The cameras were active 24 h/day with a trigger mode (Fig. 1). Cameras shot at least a burst of three pictures when triggered, with the minimal delay time possible (< 1 min) to maximize the number of samples taken per captured individual. When multiple bear photographs were taken within a 30-min interval, unless individuals were clearly distinguishable, we considered this detection as a single individual, minimizing the non-independence of consecutive pictures (Ridout and Linkie 2009; Gerber et al. 2012; Foster et al. 2013; Monterroso et al. 2014). We used the following camera models: Reconyx (RC55, HCC500, and HCC600; Reconyx, Inc., Holmen, Wisconsin); Bushnell (Trail scout, Trophy cam HD, Trophy cam XLT, Trophy cam, and Natureview; Bushnell, Kansas City, Missouri, United States); Leafriver (Trail scan and IR 7SS); and ScoutGuard (SG550, SG550V, and SG580M). We inspected and changed the batteries and memory cards every 15 to 30 days.

Identification of the detected individuals (i.e., sex and age class) was assessed by analyzing genetically hair samples collected at hair-trapping stations located in front of the camera-traps. We located one hair-trapping station at 3–5 m in front of the camera-traps. To induce bear rubbing and maximize detection probabilities, we baited the hair-trapping stations with turpentine and beechwood tar. The hair-traps consisted of tree-traps of two or three strands of barbed wire of approximately 0.3 m attached to the tree trunk and X-shaped at 0.3–1.5 m from the ground.

Data analysis

We analyzed bear activity pattern to determine the relationship with the following variables: daylight time interval; season (autumn, spring, and summer); sex class (female with offspring, solitary female, males), including adult and subadult individuals; and age class (adult: ≥ 4, subadult: < 4). We based the daylight time interval on solar time with the monthly daylight length differences and defined during equinox periods as night (1800–0559 h) and day (0600–1759 h). We estimated activity preferences at daily and seasonal scales. We conducted genetic analyses to identify the sex class and parentage assessment of the individuals, using microsatellite and SNPs markers from hair samples collected in the hair-traps. Further, we used the population genealogy to determine the age class of the detected individuals. The continuous genetic monitoring of the population since the first translocations in 1996 and 1997, together with its small size and geographical isolation, has allowed assessing paternal-offspring genealogies over time (see Quenette et al. 2018 “Suivi de l’ours brun dans les Pyrénées”, Report of the Brown Bear Transboundary Monitoring Group. Results 2018). Pyrenean brown bear population descends from translocated bears, which sex and age was known before the reintroduction. Thus, the age and parentage of the individuals have been monitored over time. We used photo-trapping methods to identify new offspring presence in the population, supporting the assessment of the population genealogy. Adult females were usually detected on the camera traps together with their offspring that remained with them during the first 2 years of rearing. Consequently, the presence of new cubs was noted. In some cases, cubs were encountered in the second year after birth. We estimated their age based on the body size of the cub when first seen on the pictures. This annual monitoring has allowed constructing the genealogy of the population, including the determination of age and sex classes. We excluded individuals with undetermined both age and sex classes. Further, to control for the differences in camera detection (see below), we used data pooling. All the analyses were restricted to data sets with > 10 activity samples. We pooled the data when activity distributions were not different across the season, daily period, sex class, and age class. To control for the experiment-wise error rate, we used an alpha level of 0.05 to determine statistical significance and applied a Bonferroni correction (Zar 1999).

We assessed daily activity patterns of bears and compared its temporal overlapping among three class groups: sex group (female with offspring vs. male, female with offspring vs. solitary female), age group (adult vs. subadult), and sex-age group (male and female with/without offspring adult vs. male and female subadult). However, due to the lower number of occurrences of juveniles, we were not able to estimate the avoidance level among seasons for the sex-age group. To evaluate the daily activity pattern, we applied the Overlap package (Meredith and Ridout 2018) for each independent detection, calculating the probability density function using the kernel density, followed by pairwise comparisons by estimating the coefficient of overlap. Overlapping coefficient (Δ) is an estimator that determines the area under the curve formed by taking the minimum of the two density functions at each time point and ranges from 0 (no overlap) to 1 (complete overlap) (Ridout and Linkie 2009; Linkie and Ridout 2011). We considered two estimators according to the sample size: Δ1 for samples < 75 and Δ4 for samples ≥ 75 (Meredith and Ridout 2018). To obtain the precision of this estimator, we computed a standard deviation from 10,000 bootstrap samples (Meredith and Ridout 2018). Thus, we used the overlapping coefficient to identify the avoidance level between the individuals, following the evaluation of Monterroso et al. (2014). We considered the following ranges of avoidance: (1) low avoidance, (2) medium avoidance, and (3) high avoidance (Δ ≥ 76, 66 ≤ Δ ≥ 75, and Δ ≤ 65, respectively).

To describe the preference for a certain time of the day or season, we conducted Rayleigh test (Z) from Circular package (Lund et al. 2017), assessing whether or not individuals displayed a random record distribution across the season and daily period (Pratas-Santiago et al. 2017). Rayleigh test considers the uniformity of a certain record distribution through that period (Zar 2010), where a higher Z value means that data are concentrated around the mean and less evenly distributed (Pratas-Santiago et al. 2017). Further, to evaluate daily and seasonal differences in the temporal patterns between coexisting pairs of sex and age bear groups, we used multiple comparison Mardia-Watson-Wheeler test (MWW) (Batschelet 1981). Statistical analyses were conducted in R software, version 3.4.3 (R Core Team 2017).

Results

A total of 2253 independent detections of brown bears were recorded from 2200 effective trapping days. Photo-trapping cameras had an average of 84.38 trapping days with bear detections (SE = 69.50) along the whole sampling period. We identified sex and age classes of the individuals in 1189 independent records. We recorded 768 independent detections of males and 421 independent detections of females (with offspring: n = 144; solitary: n = 277), both adult (n = 1112) and subadults (n = 77).

As for time-period preference, bears were detected significantly more often during the nighttime period (Rayleigh test: Z mean = 0.94, p < 0.05). The population activity pattern showed two activity peaks at 0600 h and 2200 h (> 50%), and a lower detectability (< 10%) when disturbance caused by humans is higher (1000–1800 h). The results showed differences in activity patterns between various age and sex classes, mainly demonstrated by the presence of vulnerable individuals (Figs. 3 and 4). Considering sex groups comparisons, MWW test revealed that the presence of offspring determined sex differences in daily cycle pattern (MWW test, females with offspring vs. solitary females: W = 17.06, n = 431, p = 0.0002; females with offspring vs. males: W = 36.354, n = 898, p < 0.005; males vs. solitary females: W = 4.695, n = 1049, p = 0.09). At the daily scale, females with offspring selected more diurnal periods than solitary individuals (Figs. 2 and 4), showing medium avoidance level (Δ1 ≤ 0.70). Solitary females and males showed low avoidance patterns (Δ1 ≥ 0.90). At the seasonal scale, sex groups showed high variance in detectability activity (Table 1). Generally, the number of detections was higher during spring, coinciding with increased segregation activity in all the sex groups (Fig. 4). While solitary individuals were mainly detected during the spring (females: nspring = 138; nsummer = 110; nautumn = 43; males: nspring = 439; nsummer = 257; nautumn = 62), females with offspring were detected more frequently during summer, with low intraspecific disturbance but high human presence (nspring = 24; nsummer = 82; nautumn = 34). In spring, females with offspring showed activity peaks in diurnal periods, at 0800–0900 h and 1800–2000 h, with lower activity during 0300–0500 h and 1300–1500 h. Females with offspring showed strong avoidance behavior toward males and solitary females during the nighttime (Fig. 4, Table 1). Nevertheless, solitary individuals showed lower activity in diurnal periods, at 0800–2000 h, with constant activity along nocturnal periods. In summer, while solitary individuals kept the nocturnal pattern, females with offspring increased diurnal activity at 1400–2100 h. In autumn, detectability during daylight hours (1700–2300 h) increased in all the sex classes, and females with offspring overlapped more with solitary individuals.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Percentage of independent detections of brown bear according to the sex-class (solitary females, females with offspring, and males) over a 24-h cycle (night: 1800–0559 h, day: 0600–1759 h) presented in two daily periods (day, dark gray; and night, light gray)

Table 1 Number of independent detections (n), coefficient of overlap (Δ4), and Mardia-Watson-Wheeler test (MWW) among the activity patterns of coexistent groups of sex-class of brown bears (FWO: female with offspring; SF: solitary female; M: male), on a seasonal basis (spring: with high levels of intraspecific disturbance; summer and autumn: with high levels of human disturbance), in the Pyrenees; France, Spain, and Andorra, 2010–2017

The age groups at a daily scale showed differences in temporal patterns (MWW test, adults–subadults: W = 12.58, n = 431, p = 0.0019), where subadults strongly selected diurnal periods (adult: mean ± SE = 20.21 ± 4.87; subadults: mean ± SE = 46.56 ± 9.22; Fig. 3), having a medium avoidance level of the adult bears (Δ4 = 0.70). Adult bears’ activity decreased from 0900 to 1900 h, while activity peaks of the subadult bears were between 1600 and 2000 h, just before the activity peak of adult bears and coinciding with the most human disturbance. Diurnal activity of subadult individuals was higher during the high intraspecific disturbance season, showing a higher avoidance level (Δ1 = 0.58) (MWW test, W = 14.44, n = 455, p = 0.0007).

Fig. 3
figure 3

General brown bear activity pattern and overlapping activity analysis (Δ1) in relation to the sex-age group: a adult females vs. subadult females, b adult females vs. subadult males, c adult males vs. subadult females, and d adult males vs. subadult males. Black solid lines indicate adults and dotted lines indicate subadults. The activity records for each sex-age group were nAdult F = 373; nAdult M = 739; nSubadult F = 52; nSubadult M = 25

Comparison of activity patterns of sex-age groups showed that adult males vs. subadult males (Δ1 = 0.56) and adult females vs. subadults males (Δ1 = 0.67) had the lowest overlapping coefficients. Subadult males avoided adults during higher activity peaks (at 0500–0900 h and 2000–0030 h). In contrast, the activity of subadult females was similar to the activity of adult individuals, being mainly concentrated between 1800 and 0600 h (subadult females vs. adult males: Δ1 = 0.75; and subadult females vs. adult females: Δ1 = 0.84) (Fig. 3). In general, in spring and summer, the number of detections of subadult bears of both sexes was higher than in autumn (subadult males: nspring = 12; nsummer = 10; nautumn = 2; subadult females: nspring = 26; nsummer = 21; nautumn = 7). Subadult females showed, equally among the seasons, more than 60% of the occurrences in a nocturnal period. In contrast, subadult males were more active during the day in spring and summer, while the only two occurrences recorded in autumn were nocturnal.

Discussion

Similar to other bear populations in Europe (for Croatia and Slovenia, Kaczensky et al. 2006; for Sweden, Ordiz et al. 2014), the Pyrenean bears showed high activity during nocturnal and crepuscular periods (Figs. 3 and 4). Further, females with offspring and subadults modified their nocturnal behavior to more diurnal under the presence of disturbances. In line with our expectations, solitary and adult individuals were mainly nocturnal or crepuscular, whereas vulnerable individuals (i.e., females with offspring and subadults) increased day-time activity. As we predicted based on anecdotal observations by fieldworkers in the Pyrenees (Palazón et al. unpubl. data), this behavior might be a strategy to avoid the risk of competition or predation. Daytime preference may (1) expose vulnerable individuals more frequently to humans (Kaczensky et al. 2006), especially during the high human activity periods (i.e., summer and autumn); and (2) increase random encounters with humans.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Seasonal brown bear activity pattern according to the sex classes: a the daily activity pattern during spring (nF with offspring = 24; nSolitary F = 138; nM = 439 activity records), b the daily activity pattern during summer (nF with offspring = 82; nSolitary F = 110; nM = 257 activity records), and c the daily activity pattern during autumn (nF with offspring = 34; nSolitary F = 43; nM = 62 activity records). Gray solid lines indicate females with offspring (F with offspring), black solid lines indicate males (M), and dotted lines indicate solitary females (Solitary F)

Carnivores modify their temporal activity patterns according to biological (i.e., sex or age) and external factors. This has been studied for a variety of species (e.g., Reynolds and Beecham 1980, for black bear; Foster et al. 2013, for puma and jaguar; Monterroso et al. 2014, for mesocarnivores; Munro et al. 2007; Penteriani et al. 2017; Zarzo-Arias et al. 2018, for brown bear). Anthropic pressure on European wild areas has promoted elusiveness strategies of wildlife. A known example is the variation of spatio-temporal activity responses (Ordiz et al. 2016). Most of the large carnivores in Europe have a long human persecution record. Consequently, it may have increased the vigilance and elusiveness behavior found in the current populations (Zedrosser et al. 2001; Martin et al. 2010; Ordiz et al. 2014; Hertel et al. 2016). Encounters between bears and humans in Europe have been boosted by increasing outdoor activities (Kaczensky et al. 2006; Tucker et al. 2018). Thus, human-caused disturbances, especially the use of natural areas and hunting activities, may be perceived by animals as a risk. Consequently, it may influence their spatio-temporal activity patterns (Hertel et al. 2016). In Pyrenees, in comparison with other regions of Europe, bear-targeted hunting is prohibited; however, bears may be disturbed during the hunting seasons of other groups of animals (e.g., ungulates). As a mechanism of elusiveness, individuals may change their activity to nocturnal periods.

The Pyrenean brown bear population was almost extinct in 1996, and after the reintroduction of 11 individuals, the current population descends from bears translocated from Slovenia (Piédallu et al. 2016). In our research, we assumed that relocated individuals maintained the same nocturnal activity pattern as the Slovenian population (Kaczensky et al. 2006). Many studies on brown bear behavior evidence that changes in the activity might be a result of social learning between mother and cubs (Nielsen et al. 2013; Hopkins 2013; Morehouse et al. 2016). Consequently, we consider that the nocturnal behavior of relocated females’ offspring might have been acquired through social learning during the first years of the mother-offspring relationship. Future research would further test the impact of social learning and the environment on bear cubs’ behavior in the Pyrenees.

Despite the effectiveness of adopting a nocturnal behavior as a strategy to avoid human disturbance (MacHutchon et al. 1998; Rode et al. 2006), vulnerable bears increased diurnal activity under intraspecific disturbance. Further, the major avoidance of nocturnal activity by vulnerable bears in the Pyrenees was observed during the mating season. Avoidance of intraspecific competition by vulnerable individuals has been widely described in carnivores (Steyaert et al. 2013; Wielgus et al. 2013). We believe that circadian variations in Pyrenean bears might influence intraspecific encounters. This might facilitate mating encounters or minimize competition risk (Dahle and Swenson 2003; Rode et al. 2006). Overlap of activity periods of solitary bears was higher during the mating season, possibly to increase the chances of mating encounters (Sahlén et al. 2015). However, our results showed that females with offspring and subadults were mainly detected during the day. Several reports on brown bears in Europe and North America indicated that females with offspring modify their activity to more diurnal (Gunther 1990; Klinka and Reimchen 2002). Diurnal behavior might minimize male encounters’ probabilities and infanticide risk (McLellan 2005; Bellemain et al. 2006a; Ordiz et al. 2011; Steyaert et al. 2013). With the infanticide, males create mating opportunities with victimized mothers (Steyaert et al. 2016). As a result, females evolve counterstrategies to defend their cubs (Bellemain et al. 2006b). In the Pyrenean population, non-parental infanticide was observed in two cases in 2011 and 2016 (Palazón et al. 2016). Possibly, our findings regarding the activity modulation of the mothers are the result of anti-infanticide responses during high-intensity risk periods.

Individual learning in bears provides a higher wariness toward risk situations and may be influenced by previous negative experiences. Our study showed a diurnal preference in subadult bears. It is often assumed that activity variations of inexperienced bears might be a strategy to evade competition with adult individuals, especially during their higher activity peaks. However, this activity in subadult bears is not constant over the year. Among all the subadults, females showed a low avoidance level to adult males, especially during the mating season. Low elusiveness found in subadult females may be explained by early primiparity. Early primiparity exists in brown bear females, but these cases are rare (e.g., for Sweeden, see Zedrosser et al. 2009). In the Pyrenees, there are cases of advanced primiparity cases in young females (< 4 years) (Palazón et al. unpubl. data). Furthermore, intra-sexual competition is often described in many animal groups, where dominant individuals suppress the reproduction of younger females (Støen et al. 2006). In bear populations, young females establish their home ranges close to their progenitors, increasing the chances of reproduction suppression (McLellan 2005; Bellemain et al. 2006a). Due to the small density and the large distribution range in the Pyrenees, we believe that chances of female-female encounters are low. This fact allows females to get an early reproductive maturation. Thus, activity overlapping of the subadult females with adult males might facilitate mating encounters. An alternative explanation of this activity overlapping might be that philopatric young females learn the behavioral patterns from their mothers, imitating their nocturnal activity. Diurnal preference by subadult males provides higher awareness of intraspecific risk with dominant bears. Also, subadult males tend to disperse and travel long distances (McLellan 2005; Bartoń et al. 2019), and this fact may also decrease the chances of antagonistic interactions. We have no movement data from subadult individuals in the studied population; however, we suspect that their exploratory behavior during the daylight may prevent male-male competition (Andersson 1994; Bellemain et al. 2006a).

Even if vulnerable bears reduce the negative stimuli from dominant bears, human disturbance may continue affecting diurnal individuals. The growing presence of humans in the bear-inhabited areas, especially during summer and autumn, increases chances of human-bear encounters. However, reduction of diurnal detection during autumn by all of the classes may reduce this risk. It is important to improve our knowledge on how translocated animals behave in new environments. As the behavioral strategies behind disturbance avoidance are becoming well understood, future studies might focus on other small, isolated but natural bear populations in human-modified landscapes (e.g., Zarzo-Arias et al. 2018), and help to establish optimal management interventions during translocations, human-bear conflicts prevention actions, and reduction of the risk of human-bear encounters.