Review article
Context and trade-offs characterize real-world threat detection systems: A review and comprehensive framework to improve research practice and resolve the translational crisis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.05.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • In nature, antipredator decision-making is shaped by context and characterized by trade-offs.

  • In the laboratory, neurobiological models of fear and anxiety control context and limit trade-offs.

  • Translational science is failing because models fail to address real-world conditions.

  • We develop a mechanistic model to show why contextual factors should be experimentally manipulated.

Abstract

A better understanding of context in decision-making—that is, the internal and external conditions that modulate decisions—is required to help bridge the gap between natural behaviors that evolved by natural selection and more arbitrary laboratory models of anxiety and fear. Because anxiety and fear are mechanisms evolved to manage threats from predators and other exigencies, the large behavioral, ecological and evolutionary literature on predation risk is useful for re-framing experimental research on human anxiety-related disorders. We review the trade-offs that are commonly made during antipredator decision-making in wild animals along with the context under which the behavior is performed and measured, and highlight their relevance for focused laboratory models of fear and anxiety. We then develop an integrative mechanistic model of decision-making under risk which, when applied to laboratory and field settings, should improve studies of the biological basis of normal and pathological anxiety and may therefore improve translational outcomes.

Introduction

Decision-making under risk is relevant to behavioral researchers from a variety of disciplines, including all those who study topics ranging from predator-prey interactions in the field to anxiety disorders in humans (e.g., Clinchy et al., 2011; Mobbs et al., 2018). Anxiety occurs in response to risk and is generally related to a sense of apprehension. This apprehension results, in part, because there is a conflict between a potential threat and the potential of receiving a benefit such as foraging or potential mate (McNaughton and Corr, 2004) It may be quantified as increased vigilance and can become maladaptive when it interferes with otherwise necessary behaviors that enable survival (Box 1). Risk-related decisions are thought to maximize fitness; hence, individuals that make the right decisions are those whose genes are passed on to future generations. A key insight about these decisions is the involvement of trade-offs where the costs and benefits of risky behaviors are evaluated and the optimal outcome maximizes benefits while minimizing costs. Costs primarily ensue from the risk of predation, competition, disease and parasitism (Gallagher et al., 2017). Humans, especially in modern societies, additionally live with the risks of losing social status and employment (Björkqvist, 2001). A key lesson from behavioral ecology is that in order to optimize outcomes, individuals cannot avoid all risks; by doing so it would be impossible to acquire resources or mates (Blumstein, 2008).

Both, anxiety and fear, are emotional states, associated with physiological and psychological responses. Anxiety and fear are adaptive responses if it is possible to correctly differentiate safe and threatening stimuli. If individuals are unable to distinguish between threatening and safe stimuli, anxiety and fear may become maladaptive. If such a maladaptive state lasts longer, it becomes pathological and in humans we might diagnose an anxiety disorder. Decisions about risks may vary according to a range of internal factors (Kiyokawa et al., 2009) and the external environment (Campbell-Palmer and Rosell, 2011; Orrock and Danielson, 2009) at the time a threat is detected. For example, moon phase is known to influence antipredator decision-making in several prey species. Oldfield mice (Peromyscus polionotus) and woodmice (Apodemus sylvaticus) are more likely to respond to predator cues on full-moon nights when the mice are most visible, and hence vulnerable, to their predators (Orrock and Danielson, 2009; Orrock et al., 2004). By contrast tammar wallabies (Notamacropus eugenii) increase the time spent foraging under moonlight, suggesting that they feel safer under illumination when high visibility improves their ability to detect predators (Biebouw and Blumstein, 2003). Humans are also influenced by moonlight. In Tanzania, where lion attacks are common, the full-moon causes anxiety among people (Packer et al., 2011), ostensibly because the coming nights will be darkest. Among non-vertebrates, European leeches (Hirudo verbana) modify their behavior according to how deep they are within water, and whether they have been fed a blood meal (Palmer et al., 2014; Palmer and Kristan, 2011). Thus, there are a variety of ways that context (light, depth of water, or a meal in these cases) modifies decision-making based on the assessment of these factors.

Consequently, there is widespread recognition that trade-offs, and the context in which they are made, must be incorporated into models of decision-making (Caro, 2005; Lima, 1998). However, not enough laboratory research considers these cost-benefit trade-offs and contextual variables that characterize anxiety and fear in nature (Gray and McNaughton, 2000; McNaughton and Corr, 2004). This is especially important if we are to make progress in solving a crisis in translational biomedical research (Manjili, 2013) where results from preclinical studies cannot approximate pharmacological effects in clinical studies or real-world settings (Kinsella and Monk, 2009; Oppenheim, 2019), or similarly, in conservation and wildlife management where fear-based management tools cannot approximate outcomes from fear cues in the laboratory (Parsons et al., 2018).

For instance, an isolated laboratory rodent might be exposed to a specific stimulus that could generate fear or anxiety, but the type and extent of its behavioral response is shaped by other factors, such as the animal’s current satiety status (internal drive; Lõhmus and Sundström, 2004) or the presence of further potential dangers (external context; Nersesian et al., 2012; Orrock et al., 2004; Parsons and Blumstein, 2010). Despite this potential of contextual variables to profoundly influence behavior (Wolff, 2003), many such variables are sacrificed in laboratory/neurobiological models specifically to improve power and replicability (Klumpers and Kroes, 2019). The resulting animal models allow us to use powerful approaches such as optogenetics and chemogenetics to understand neurobiological mechanisms underlying decision-making under risk. They also allow us to manipulate internal and external conditions we wish to understand, but cannot be directly manipulated in humans. Yet, these models are not intended to account for the influence of context in real-world decision making.

Here, we review and discuss the trade-offs that should be taken into account during antipredator decision-making in wild animals, and integrate them with neurobiological models of fear and anxiety. We emphasize the importance of internal drives and external contexts in decision-making via an integrative mechanistic model, for which we have summarized the neural basis for threat-induced defensive behaviors. While calls for real world models have been increasing over the past decade (Kinsella and Monk, 2009; Oppenheim, 2019), our approach is novel in that we develop a more integrative model that shows how decision-making in response to conflicting internal drives is influenced by fear and may result in anxiety. Lastly, following our review, we propose how decision-making research can be improved, and thereby facilitate new semi-realistic and naturalistic approaches. These outcomes are intended to address the growing bench-to-bedside gap in translational medicine (Manjili, 2013), while also enhancing wildlife conservation and management.

Section snippets

Benefits and costs of controlled laboratory experiments

Neuroethological laboratory experiments are often performed under standardized conditions where it is typical to modify only one critical variable while all others are fixed. Standardization can include the use of inbred strains of rodents and investigators’ clothes behavior (Sorge et al., 2014), use of skincare products (Brower et al., 1998), sex, and reproductive status (Vaclavik et al., 2012), as well as user–not animal–defined timelines (Bruinsma et al., 2019). Nevertheless, attempts to

Towards an integrative mechanistic model of decisions made under risk

Behavioral decisions such as where to forage, when to rest, and with whom to mate all have consequences (Suraci et al., 2016). A large body of literature already considers decisions made under the risk of predation (Caro, 2005; Lima and Dill, 1990). Contemporary humans are not usually exposed directly to predation (but see Gurung et al., 2008; Packer et al., 2011), but are threatened by other factors including social pressure (Gurung et al., 2008; Stein and Stein, 2008), work or financial

A comprehensive model of decision-making in response to conflicting internal drives

We next introduce a comprehensive model showing how the pattern of ID and EC may modulate the emotional state in Fig. 1. In natural settings, an individual usually detects multiple stimuli concurrently (Fig. 2A left). Although effects of each stimulus are analyzed individually in the laboratory (Fig. 2A middle), behavioral responses are determined by the combination of detected stimuli (Fig. 2A right). In the field of ethology, these questions are mostly analyzed by asking how EC affects

An appeal for more integrative studies of decisions made under risk

This integrative, mechanistic model guides us to identify key contextual factors that influence experimental results in the laboratory and field, under natural and pathological conditions. For instance, returning to Fig. 1, we can now appreciate how wild animals or humans with anxiety are affected in their decision making: low(er) risk cues may trigger higher risk emotional states (Fig. 1: red-dashed arrows, middle part of figure) and hence costly behavioral and physiological responses (Fig. 1:

Declaration of Competing Interest

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgements

This review emerged from a workshop “Avoiding danger and searching for safety: From predator-prey interactions in the field to anxiety disorders in humans” at the Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany, that was supported by the State of Saxony-Anhalt & European Regional Development Fund (Center of Behavioral Brain Sciences; FKZ: ZS/2016/04/78113), the German Science Foundation (SFB779, FE483/8-1) and the Zwillenberg-Tietz foundation. DTB is supported by the Australian Research Council

References (130)

  • A.J. Gallagher et al.

    Energy landscapes and the landscape of fear

    Trends Ecol. Evol.

    (2017)
  • T.D. Goode et al.

    Threat imminence dictates the role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in contextual fear

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2020)
  • B. Gurung et al.

    Factors associated with human-killing tigers in Chitwan National Park, Nepal

    Biol. Conserv.

    (2008)
  • M.A. Hagenaars et al.

    Updating freeze: aligning animal and human research

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2014)
  • M.H.M. Hutschemaekers et al.

    Endogenous testosterone levels are predictive of symptom reduction with exposure therapy in social anxiety disorder

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2020)
  • N. Kirlic et al.

    Animal to human translational paradigms relevant for approach avoidance conflict decision making

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2017)
  • F. Klumpers et al.

    S7. Experimentally assessing costly fearful avoidance and its relation to anxious psychophysiology

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2018)
  • S. Lakehayli et al.

    Prenatal stress alters sensitivity to benzodiazepines in adult rats

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2015)
  • M. Lõhmus et al.

    Leptin and social environment influence the risk-taking and feeding behaviour of Asian blue quail

    Anim. Behav.

    (2004)
  • W. Loughry et al.

    Calling and vigilance in California ground squirrels: a test of the tonic communication hypothesis

    Anim. Behav.

    (1988)
  • R.P. Mady et al.

    Social security: are socially connected individuals less vigilant?

    Anim. Behav.

    (2017)
  • T.J. McDermott et al.

    Roadmap for optimizing the clinical utility of emotional stress paradigms in human neuroimaging research

    Neurobiol. Stress

    (2018)
  • N. McNaughton et al.

    A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2004)
  • V. Micale et al.

    Endocannabinoid system and mood disorders: priming a target for new therapies

    Pharmacol. Ther.

    (2013)
  • M.J. Millan

    The neurobiology and control of anxious states

    Prog. Neurobiol.

    (2003)
  • D. Mobbs et al.

    Space, time, and fear: survival computations along defensive circuits

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2020)
  • M.S. Mooring et al.

    Costs of allogrooming in impala: distraction from vigilance

    Anim. Behav.

    (1995)
  • M.N. Muller et al.

    Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge’ hypothesis

    Anim. Behav.

    (2004)
  • S.M. O’Mahony et al.

    Early life stress alters behavior, immunity, and microbiota in rats: implications for irritable bowel syndrome and psychiatric illnesses

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • C.R. Palmer et al.

    Contextual modulation of behavioral choice

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2011)
  • S. Patel et al.

    The endocannabinoid system as a target for novel anxiolytic drugs

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2017)
  • A.G. Pereira et al.

    Is there anybody out there? Neural circuits of threat detection in vertebrates

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2016)
  • E. Phelps et al.

    Neural systems underlying emotion behavior: from animal models to human function

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • W. Pisula et al.

    Response to novelty in the laboratory Wistar rat, wild-captive WWCPS rat, and the gray short-tailed opossum (Monodelphis domestica)

    Behav. Processes

    (2012)
  • C. Poirotte et al.

    Morbid attraction to leopard urine in Toxoplasma-infected chimpanzees

    Curr. Biol.

    (2016)
  • V. Aho et al.

    Homeostatic response to sleep/rest deprivation by constant water flow in larval zebrafish in both dark and light conditions

    J. Sleep Res.

    (2017)
  • P.B. Banks et al.

    Olfaction and Predator-Prey Interactions amongst Mammals in Australia. Carnivores of Australia: Past, Present and Future

    (2014)
  • G. Beauchamp

    Sleeping gulls monitor the vigilance behaviour of their neighbours

    Biol. Lett.

    (2008)
  • A.J. Bechtholt et al.

    Anxiolytic effect of serotonin depletion in the novelty-induced hypophagia test

    Psychopharmacology

    (2007)
  • M. Ben-Moussa et al.

    DJINNI: a novel technology supported exposure therapy paradigm for SAD combining virtual reality and augmented reality

    Front. Psychiatry

    (2017)
  • K. Biebouw et al.

    Tammar wallabies (Macropus eugenii) associate safety with higher levels of nocturnal illumination

    Ethol. Ecol. Evol.

    (2003)
  • O. Bimber et al.

    Spatial Augmented Reality: Merging Real and Virtual Worlds

    (2005)
  • D.T. Blumstein

    Fourteen lessons from anti-predator behavior

  • D.T. Blumstein et al.

    Olfactory predator recognition: wallabies may have to learn to be wary

    Anim. Conserv.

    (2002)
  • D.T. Blumstein et al.

    Social security: social relationship strength and connectedness influence how marmots respond to alarm calls

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. (Print)

    (2017)
  • M.T. Bowen et al.

    Oxytocin and vasopressin modulate the social response to threat: a preclinical study

    Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (2014)
  • J.E. Brower et al.

    Field and Laboratory Methods for General Ecology

    (1998)
  • M. Browning et al.

    Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2015)
  • B. Bruinsma et al.

    An automated home-cage-based 5-choice serial reaction time task for rapid assessment of attention and impulsivity in rats

    Psychopharmacology

    (2019)
  • K.A. Byers et al.

    A novel method for affixing global positioning system (GPS) tags to urban Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus): feasibility, health impacts and potential for tracking movement

    J. Urban Ecol.

    (2017)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    Authors contributed equally.

    2

    Current affiliation: Boehringer Ingelheim Pharma GmbH & Co KG, Biberach, Germany.

    View full text