Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 151, 1 August 2020, 104713
Appetite

Modification of food preferences by posthypnotic suggestions: An event-related brain potential study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104713Get rights and content

Abstract

The preference for high-over low-calorie food and difficulties in inhibiting the desire for high-calorie food are important factors involved in unhealthy food choices. Here, we explored posthypnotic suggestions (PHS), aiming to increase the desire for vegetables and fruits, as a possible new tool to induce a preference for low-calorie food. Following the termination of hypnosis, PHS was activated and deactivated in counterbalanced order, while event-related brain-potentials were recorded. Two tasks were administered, a food-face classification measuring implicit food preferences, where stimuli were categorized as showing food items or faces, and a Go-NoGo task measuring inhibition, where food items were selected as being appropriate for making a salad or not. In the food-face classification task without PHS, the early visual P1 component, a marker of stimulus reward-associations, was larger in response to high-than low-calorie food pictures; PHS eliminated this difference. PHS also yielded faster RTs and larger amplitudes of a late positive component in low-versus high-calorie items. Hence, PHS appeared to neutralize the positive perceptual bias toward high-calorie food items and enhance the effective processing of low-calorie items by increasing motivated attention. In the Go-NoGo task, PHS decreased the NoGo-N2; PHS increased the early Go- and NoGo-P3, possibly by turning low- and high-calorie items more pleasant and unpleasant, respectively, requiring more proactive control to inhibit task-irrelevant food-related emotions. Further, in the Go condition, PHS quickened the rejection of salad-inappropriate high-calorie items and increased the amplitude of late-P3, indicating facilitated classification of high-calorie items and increased response monitoring. Together, PHS effectively increased the preference for low-calorie food and the inhibition of impulses toward high-calorie food; therefore, PHS may be a promising tool for supporting healthy and sustainable food choices.

Introduction

Unhealthy food preferences and food choices contribute to the global burden of disease and environmental sustainability (Clark, Springmann, Hill, & Tilman, 2019; Forouzanfar et al., 2015; Haddad et al., 2016). For example, the number of obese and overweight individuals with a body mass index (BMI) > 25 has grown steadily in most countries (Rodgers, Woodward, Swinburn, & Dietz, 2018). Traditional measures have fallen short of stopping the obesity epidemic (OECD, 2017) and have yet to show their effectiveness to change diets towards healthier and more environmentally friendly patterns as suggested by the Lancet commission (Willett et al., 2019). Therefore, novel tools are needed to help individuals to shift their food preferences towards healthier choices and resist temptations by unhealthy options. The present study explores posthypnotic suggestions (PHS) as a possible way to modify food preferences. Of the plethora of factors determining food consumption (e.g., Stok et al., 2017), we targeted two psychological variables, food preferences and the inhibition function (Guerrieri, Nederkoorn, & Jansen, 2008; Nederkoorn, Guerrieri, Havermans, Roefs, & Jansen, 2009; Nederkoorn, Houben, Hofmann, Roefs, & Jansen, 2010.)

Preferences for fatty, sweet, or salty food, are central in determining the health outcomes of diets (e.g., Clark et al., 2019) and the global burden of disease (Haddad et al., 2016). For example, obese individuals tend to consume more high-calorie food, rich in sugar and fat in comparison to individuals with normal weight (Ebbeling et al., 2004; Schrauwen & Westerterp, 2000; Seidell, 1998). Food preferences are already influenced in utero and during breastfeeding by the mother's diet (Maier-Noth, 2019; Wilson, 2015) but are subject to alteration or entrenchment throughout life (Emond et al., 2019).

Unhealthy preferences are reinforced by the ubiquitous availability of these kinds of food in modern affluent societies. For some individuals overcoming the negative influences of unhealthy food preferences is more difficult than for others. Thus, food consumption is related to the inhibition (Dohle, Diel, & Hofmann, 2018), the executive function (EF) responsible for the suppression of prepotent but inappropriate responses (Diamond, 2013; Miyake et al., 2000). Corroborating this notion, Nederkoorn et al. (2009) observed that implicit preferences for high-calorie food are most detrimental in individuals with low inhibition abilities. In a meta-analysis Yang, Shields, Guo, and Liu (2018) reported significant deficits in inhibition and EFs in over-weight and obese individuals. Blocking EFs with transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) increased consumption of snack food (Lowe, Hall, & Staines, 2014). In adolescents, poor EFs are a predictor of obesity (Tee, Gan, Tan, & Chin, 2018). Finally, individuals with higher BMI showed less efficient EFs (e.g., Prickett, Brennan, & Stolwyk, 2015; Smith, Hay, Campbell, & Trollor, 2011), especially in terms of inhibition (Bartholdy et al., 2017). Therefore, in addition to food preferences, inhibition of reflexive but inappropriate desires appears to be an important factor in food choice.

In the present study, we assessed the effects of PHS, tailored to increase the preference for healthy food and – by implied contrast – inhibit impulses toward high-calorie food. That means, even though we did not expect participants to reject or devalue high-calorie food items as our PHS targeted low-calorie food (fruits, vegetables), it seemed conceivable that participants inhibited their desire for high-calorie items. To understand the mechanisms of PHSs one should consider that hypnosis is a state of consciousness with three central properties, concentration on oneself, dissociation from the surroundings and increased suggestibility (Green, Barabasz, Barrett, & Montgomery, 2005). PHSs are presented during the hypnotic state but will be activated only after the termination of hypnosis by a specific cue such as an associated hand gesture. Multiple studies have shown that, rather than abandoning self-control, hypnosis directs and implements more effectively one's cognitive control repertoire (e.g., Iani, Ricci, Gherri, & Rubichi, 2006; Raz et al., 2003; Raz, Fan, & Posner, 2005; Sheehan, Donovan, & MacLeod, 1988; Zahedi, Stuermer, Abdel Rahman, & Sommer, 2019; Zahedi, Stuermer, Hatami, Rostami, & Sommer, 2017), and may work like a very efficient form of mental practice (Zahedi et al. in prep). In cognitive behavior therapy hypnosis is frequently used to change cognitive biases and preferences towards particular types of stimuli or mental contents (Hertel & Mathews, 2011; Kihlstrom, 2014; Kirsch, Montgomery, & Sapirstein, 1995; Milburn, 2010). Also, it is shown that hypnosis can affect perception; for instance, visual perception (Schmidt, Hecht, Naumann, & Miltner, 2017) and pain perception (Perri, Rossani, & Di Russo, 2019) have been modified successfully with hypnosis; interestingly, in both studies, the changes in perception were related to changes in brain activities, such as the P3 component. Although, to our knowledge, no previous study has applied PHSs to food preferences, in children they can be modified already by simple stories presented outside of any hypnotic context (Duncker, 1938) and also by food advertisements (Emond et al., 2019). It is important to consider that hypnotic-like experiences are defined as conditions where someone is concentrated on a special object and dissociated from other objects, such as in reading an engaging book or watching TV (Shor & Orne, 1962). In this sense, advertisements might be even more similar to hypnosis as they may provoke not only concentration and dissociation but also aim to induce increased suggestibility. Further, in an interesting study, Ludwig et al. (2014) used PHSs to induce disgust toward pictures containing different food categories, when they were superimposed on a background with a specific color, such as green or red. Notably, their PHS was not directed toward the food stimuli themselves but to the background color. The changes in the perception were correlated with decreased activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), possibly showing that PHSs caused participants to devalue objects suggested to be disgusting. Therefore, we expected PHSs to be capable of changing food preferences.

In order to assess the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the changes obtained by PHSs, we measured event-related brain potentials (ERPs) derived from the EEG elicited by pictures of food. ERPs are a valuable addition to behavioral measures because they provide insight into the cognitive processes mediating between stimuli and responses. Especially as it has been shown that different ERP component, such as P1, N1, P3, and late positivity complex (Allen, Iacono, Laravuso, & Dunn, 1995; Terhune, Cardena, & Lindgren, 2010; Zahedi et al., 2019) were modulated by PHSs in different tasks. Of special interest for the present study was the effect of different categories of depicted food on certain ERP components. The first components of interest were the early visual components P1 and N1, likely generated in extrastriata and inferotemporal cortex, respectively (e.g., Hickey, Chelazzi, & Theeuwes, 2010; Meule, Kubler, & Blechert, 2013; Toepel, Knebel, Hudry, le Coutre, & Murray, 2009). P1 amplitude has been reported to be larger to reward-associated in comparison to neutral or punishment-associated stimuli (Hickey et al., 2010; Schacht, Adler, Chen, Guo, & Sommer, 2012), and was positively correlated with craving for the presented stimuli in smokers (Donohue et al., 2016). Pictures of high-calorie/high-fat food has been reported to elicit a smaller N1 component (150–200 ms) than their counterparts in the studies of Meule et al. (2013) and Toepel et al. (2009).

Another component of interest is the late parietal positivity (LPP) which increases to affective relative to neutral stimuli, and is attributed to motivated attention directed at these items (Schupp, Flaisch, Stockburger, & Junghofer, 2006). LPPs to pictures of food have been reported to be larger than to non-food items, to increase as function of hunger (Nijs, Franken, & Muris, 2008; Nijs, Muris, Euser, & Franken, 2010; Stockburger, Schmalzle, Flaisch, Bublatzky, & Schupp, 2009; Stockburger, Weike, Hamm, & Schupp, 2008), of immediate or delayed consumption (Meule et al., 2013), and whether the depicted food was edible or rotten (Becker, Flaisch, Renner, & Schupp, 2016). Therefore, early and late ERP components appear to be suitable measures of the immediate significance (early components) of food or motivated attention directed at them (LPP).

Some food-related ERP studies tapped into self-regulation or EFs. Thinking about short- or long-term consequences of high- or low-calorie food pictures yielded a positive correlation between emotional eating and the LPP (Meule et al., 2013). Deliberately increasing or decreasing appetite for high-calorie food pictures affected long-latency but not earlier ERP components (Sarlo, Ubel, Leutgeb, & Schienle, 2013). Further, in Go-NoGo tasks, the amplitude of the N2 component to NoGo stimuli – taken as a sign of conflict (Enriquez-Geppert, Konrad, Pantev, & Huster, 2010; Liu, Xiao, & Shi, 2017) - was larger when food rather than non-food items served as NoGo stimuli (Watson & Garvey, 2013) and the N2 amplitude predicted the amount of food consumed after the experiment (Carbine et al., 2017).

For assessing food preferences and EFs, we utilized a face-food classification and a Go-NoGo task, respectively. In the former task, pictures of different food items were presented intermixed with pictures of faces, while participants should classify these two picture categories by choice-response button presses. This calorie-unrelated task of explicitly classifying stimuli into food or face categories (Fig. S.4 in supplementary materials), aimed to distract participants from developing hypotheses regarding the experimenters’ intentions and to measure implicit changes in food preference induced by the PHS. As the specific properties of food pictures, such as calorie content or tastiness of the depicted food items, are irrelevant for accomplishing this task, it can be considered to tap into implicit food preferences. As our PHS aimed to render low-calorie items more desirable and attractive, we expected faster responses to the low-calorie items during the PHS-active than in the PHS-inactive condition; no response-related PHS effects were anticipated for high-calorie food items. In the ERPs of the food-face classification task, we expected PHSs-related modulations of P1 and LPP components, indicating alterations in reward-associations and motivated attention, especially to low-calorie items, respectively.

In the Go-NoGo task, participants were instructed to press a button in response to frequent high-calorie food items (Go responses) and withhold their response to infrequent low-calorie items (NoGo condition). NoGo trials are considered to challenge the inhibition function (Jr & Pennington, 1996; Miyake et al., 2000; Roberts; Weisbrod, Kiefer, Marzinzik, & Spitzer, 2000), because during them a frequent and therefore prepotent response shall be withheld. The NoGo-P3 component has been consistently related to inhibition, but the NoGo-N2 was related to different factors, such as response inhibition, conflict monitoring, or emotion regulation (e.g., Albert, Lopez-Martin, & Carretie, 2010; Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2010; Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2013; Liu et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2014; Zhao, Lin, Xie, & Liu, 2019). Notably, emotional stimuli may disrupt the inhibition process (e.g., Rebetez, Rochat, Billieux, Gay, & Van der Linden, 2015; Schulz et al., 2007; Verbruggen & De Houwer, 2007) and affect both N2 and P3 components. Yang et al. (2014) reported, when NoGo stimuli were emotional facial expressions – either positive or negative – rather than neutral expressions, response time to Go stimuli and NoGo-N2 amplitudes decreased. In addition, P3 amplitudes to emotional facial expressions, when serving as Go or NoGo stimuli were bigger than to neutral expressions. Zhao et al. (2019) found that task-irrelevant emotionally positive background pictures reduced the NoGo-N2 amplitude relative to neutral pictures and increased the NoGo-P3, even after strictly controlling for arousal in the emotionally loaded stimuli.

Our PHS aimed to change low-calorie food items into the one and only desirable food category, hence inducing positive emotions toward these items and likely reducing the desirability of high-calorie food items, possibly inducing negative emotions. Therefore, we expected activated PHS to affect both Go and NoGo conditions. Specifically, without PHS we expected pronounced conflicts when the frequent button-pressing response was to be inhibited and, therefore, a salient NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 (Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2010; Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2013). However, in the PHS-active condition, as NoGo stimuli should become more desirable and pleasant we expected a smaller NoGo-N2 and an increased NoGo-P3 (Albert et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2014; Zhao et al., 2019). Simultaneously, as in PHS-active condition high-calorie items should become less desirable or even aversive, we expected an increased Go-P3.

Section snippets

Participants

Although many hypnosis studies include only highly hypnotizable individuals (e.g., Augustinova & Ferrand, 2012; Raz et al., 2003; Raz et al., 2005; Zahedi et al., 2017; Zahedi et al., 2019), testing also medium hypnotizable participants, facilitates generalizing the effects (Jensen et al., 2017). Therefore, in the current study, we included both medium and highly hypnotizable individuals, based on the Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, form-A (Shor & Orne, 1962, 1963) translated to

Performance

Food and face stimuli were analyzed separately because the experimental factors differed. ANOVA of RTs to food stimuli (Fig. 1A and B) revealed a main effect of calorie content, F(1,18)=4.9,p<.05,ηp2=.21, an interaction of calorie content and PHS, F(1,18)=5.1,p<.05,ηp2=.22, and a three-way interaction between calorie content, PHS and order of PHS activation, F(1,18)=6.1,p<.05,ηp2=.25. Planned post-hoc t-tests of the two-way interaction between calorie content and PHS, showed no calorie effect

Discussion

By employing two tasks and ERP recordings, we explored the effects of PHS to increase the value of low-calorie food over high-calorie food by inducing craving and desire for salads, fruit and vegetables in response to pictures of these food items. A food-face classification task did not involve calorie content as task-relevant dimension but required the classification of pictures as representing food or faces. A Go-NoGo task explicitly required the categorization of food items as being suitable

Conclusions

In conclusion, the results of the food-face classification and the Go-NoGo task show clear and specific effects of the food-type related PHSs. The food-face classification task can be considered as implicit with respect to the calorie dimension as it was task-irrelevant. By implementing PHS, suggesting desiring low-calorie food, an increase in implicit preference for low-calorie items and hence, reduction of an advantage for high-calorie-food at the early visual processing level was observed,

Ethical statement

The study had been approved by the ethics committee of the Institut für Psychologie of the Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin. Prior to the experiment, signed consent had been obtained. Participation was compensated either with 8 € per hour or course credits.

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by a scholarship to A.Z. by the Deutscher Akademischer Austauschdienst (DAAD). We thank Daniela Edreva and Lukas Wiegmann for help in data acquisition and Ulrike Bunzenthal, Rainer Kniesche, and Thomas Pinkpank for technical support.

The tasks and data of the current study is available in the following repository:

https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/SWP2A.

References (93)

  • P.D. Gajewski et al.

    Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2013)
  • C. Iani et al.

    Attention control and susceptibility to hypnosis

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2009)
  • W.D.S. Killgore et al.

    Cortical and limbic activation during viewing of high- versus low-calorie foods

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • K.B. Klein et al.

    Modulation of gastric acid secretion by hypnosis

    Gastroenterology

    (1989)
  • A. Miyake et al.

    The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis

    Cognitive Psychology

    (2000)
  • I.M. Nijs et al.

    Food cue-elicited brain potentials in obese and healthy-weight individuals

    Eating Behaviors

    (2008)
  • I.M. Nijs et al.

    Differences in attention to food and food intake between overweight/obese and normal-weight females under conditions of hunger and satiety

    Appetite

    (2010)
  • R.L. Perri et al.

    Neuroelectric evidences of top-down hypnotic modulation associated with somatosensory processing of sensory and limbic regions

    NeuroImage

    (2019)
  • J. Polich

    Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • C. Prickett et al.

    Examining the relationship between obesity and cognitive function: A systematic literature review

    Obesity Research & Clinical Practice

    (2015)
  • A. Raz et al.

    Posthypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop interference under cycloplegia

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2003)
  • A. Rodgers et al.

    Prevalence trends tell us what did not precipitate the US obesity epidemic

    The Lancet Public Health

    (2018)
  • M. Sarlo et al.

    Cognitive reappraisal fails when attempting to reduce the appetitive value of food: An ERP study

    Biological Psychology

    (2013)
  • A. Schacht et al.

    Association with positive outcome induces early effects in event-related brain potentials

    Biological Psychology

    (2012)
  • K.P. Schulz et al.

    Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog

    Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

    (2007)
  • H.T. Schupp et al.

    Emotion and attention: Event-related brain potential studies

    Progress in Brain Research

    (2006)
  • J.C. Seidell

    Dietary fat and obesity: An epidemiologic perspective

    American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (1998)
  • J. Stockburger et al.

    The impact of hunger on food cue processing: An event-related brain potential study

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • D.B. Terhune et al.

    Disruption of synaesthesia by posthypnotic suggestion: An ERP study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • U. Toepel et al.

    The brain tracks the energetic value in food images

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • T.D. Watson et al.

    Neurocognitive correlates of processing food-related stimuli in a Go/No-go paradigm

    Appetite

    (2013)
  • M. Weisbrod et al.

    Executive control is disturbed in schizophrenia: Evidence from event-related potentials in a go/NoGo task

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • W. Willett et al.

    Food in the anthropocene: The EAT–lancet commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems

    The Lancet

    (2019)
  • Y. Yang et al.

    Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis and review

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2018)
  • D. Zhao et al.

    Emotional arousal elicited by irrelevant stimuli affects event-related potentials (ERPs) during response inhibition

    Physiology & Behavior

    (2019)
  • J.J. Allen et al.

    An event-related potential investigation of posthypnotic recognition amnesia

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1995)
  • M. Augustinova et al.

    Suggestion does not de-automatize word reading: Evidence from the semantically based stroop task

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (2012)
  • C.A. Becker et al.

    Neural correlates of the perception of spoiled food stimuli

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • J. Blechert et al.

    Food-pics: A database of free images of food for experimental research

  • W. Bongartz

    German norms for the harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, form A

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis

    (1985)
  • K.A. Carbine et al.

    Testing food-related inhibitory control to high- and low-calorie food stimuli: Electrophysiological responses to high-calorie food stimuli predict calorie and carbohydrate intake

    Psychophysiology

    (2017)
  • M.A. Clark et al.

    Multiple health and environmental impacts of foods

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2019)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences {Ind cd

    (1988)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2016)
  • A. Diamond

    Executive functions

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2013)
  • S.E. Donohue et al.

    An electrophysiological dissociation of craving and stimulus-dependent attentional capture in smokers

    Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • Cited by (14)

    • Capsaicin-induced pain increases neural responses to low-calorie non-spicy food cues: An ERP study

      2022, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Enhanced visual attention to food images may potentially influence individuals’ food preferences (Velazquez & Pasch, 2014) and food choices (Doolan et al., 2014; Manippa et al., 2018). To date, food stimuli employed in previous studies only considered a single dimension, such as high-calorie versus low-calorie (Dimitropoulos et al., 2012; Manippa et al., 2018; Zahedi et al., 2020), or spicy versus non-spicy (Wang et al., 2020; Zhou, Gao, Small, & Chen, 2019) food images. Actually, food energy and food spiciness cross each other in daily life.

    • Using event-related potentials to study food-related cognition: An overview of methods and perspectives for future research

      2022, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Another interesting line of research would be to study the effects of targeted interventions on the brain processing of food stimuli. For example, Zahedi et al. (2020) tested whether posthypnotic suggestions could increase the desire for low-calorie foods such as vegetables and fruits. By measuring several ERP components (e.g. P100, LPP, N200, P300), they showed that posthypnotic suggestions reduced the perceptual bias towards high-calorie foods and increased motivated attention towards low-calorie foods (Zahedi et al., 2020).

    • Can posthypnotic suggestions boost updating in working memory? Behavioral and ERP evidence

      2020, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the present study, we assessed a novel approach to enhance updating in WM by applying posthypnotic suggestions (PHS). Even though PHSs can enhance performance in various cognitive tasks, such as Stroop (e.g., Parris et al., 2012; Raz et al., 2005; Raz et al., 2006; Zahedi et al., 2019; Zahedi et al., 2017), Eriksen (Iani et al., 2006), Simon (Iani et al., 2009), and Go-NoGo tasks (Zahedi et al., 2020), the underlying mechanisms of these enhancements are not well known. To our knowledge, most published experimental studies applying PHSs have focused on tasks capitalizing on the inhibition function (for review please see Lifshitz et al., 2013).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text