Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Countertransference feelings and personality disorders: a psychometric evaluation of a brief version of the Feeling Word Checklist (FWC-BV)

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

The Feeling Word Checklist (FWC) is a self-report questionnaire designed to assess therapists’ countertransference (CT) feelings. The primary aim of the study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of a brief, 12-item version of the Feeling Word Checklist (FWC-BV). The second aim was to validate the factor structure by examining the associations between the FWC-BV factors, patients’ personality pathology and therapeutic alliance (TA).

Methods

Therapists at 13 different outpatient units within the Norwegian Network of Personality Disorders participated, and the study includes therapies for a large sample of patients (N = 2425) with personality pathology. Over a period of 2.5 years, therapists completed the FWC-BV for each patient in therapy every 6 months. Statistical methods included exploratory (EFA) and confirmatory (CFA) factor analysis. Internal consistency was estimated using Mc Donald’s coefficient Omega (ωt). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV – Axis II (SCID II) and Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) were used as diagnostic instruments, and patient-rated TA was assessed using the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI-SR).

Results

Factor analyses revealed three clinically meaningful factors: Inadequate, Idealised and Confident. These factors had acceptable psychometric properties. Most notably, a number of borderline PD criteria correlated positively with the factors Inadequate and Idealised, and negatively with the factor Confident. All the factors correlated significantly with at least one of the WAI-SR subscales.

Conclusions

The FWC-BV measures three clinically meaningful aspects of therapists’ CT feelings. This brief version of the FWC seems satisfactory for use in further research and in clinical contexts.

Peer Review reports

Background

Sigmund Freud first introduced the term countertransference (CT) to refer to an analyst’s transference to the patient [1]. That is, therapists unconsciously displace feelings from their past onto analytic situations. Ideally, this is not supposed to happen; analysts are supposed to stay calm and objective, allowing no personal material to interfere with therapy. In this narrow Freudian view, CT is essentially an obstacle to be overcome, arising from therapists’ own unresolved conflicts. Freud’s solution was for therapists to undergo more analyses to become aware of and gain control over such potentially disturbing feelings. In the 1950s, the concept of CT broadened. It was now seen as a road to knowledge about the patients’ problems, as patients communicate something important about their inner world with the feelings they induce in their therapist. In this broader view, CT is understood as all the feelings evoked in the therapist, both conscious and unconscious [2, 3]. Later, the concept has been further modified with what is referred to as relational psychoanalysis and is here seen more as a co-created phenomenon [4,5,6]. According to Gabbard [7, 8], CT has expanded to encapsulate both the narrow and the broad view. However, there is still a controversy within the psychoanalytic community as to what countertransference is and what role it plays during treatment. In this study, CT is defined as the therapist’s emotional response to the patient, that is, the feelings evoked when treating a patient.

Although the concept of CT originally derives from psychoanalytic theory, several current psychotherapeutic approaches regard the therapist’s emotional reactions as an important aspect of the therapeutic process [9,10,11]. However, historically, empirical work on CT has been sparse compared to the enormous amount of theoretical literature written about the phenomenon. A main challenge in the development of systematic research has been the lack of a common definition of CT. Additionally, CT is conceptualized as a partially unconscious process, and therefore difficult to assess. What can be measured are its manifestations. In the present study, we used a questionnaire (FWC-BV) designed to capture the therapist’s self-reported feelings. That is, the feelings and experiences the therapists become aware of, acknowledge, remember and are willing to report after sessions [12]. With self-reports, we only get access to the conscious manifestations of CT. Thus, we might only capture a small part of the CT phenomenon with this method. From a psychoanalytic perspective, it is theorized that it is the feelings that we are not aware of that often drive us to act. Patients with personality disorders (PD) may particularly evoke strong emotions in the therapist and in severe cases CT can contribute to derailing treatment. However, clinical experience suggests that by focusing on CT, we can see that the same patterns in the therapeutic dyad are repeated over and over again. The therapist may then gradually become more aware of CT [12]. Despite the obvious weakness that self-reports only capture the conscious aspects of CT, recent studies have concluded that self-reports can measure CT phenomena in clinically sophisticated and psychometrically sound ways [9, 13, 14]. Additionally, questionnaires can provide a method of capturing information about the treatment process, which may help clinicians make diagnostic and therapeutic use of their own response to the patient [9, 13, 14].

Two approaches have been used to measure CT empirically. One is to have therapists fill out self-report questionnaires [13, 15]. The other is to have an external observer evaluate recorded material from sessions [16,17,18]. An advantage of self-reports is their quantitative nature; they can be distributed to many therapists, and one can subsequently aggregate large amounts of data enabling identification of common patterns of feelings [9]. The Feeling Word Checklist (FWC) [15, 19, 20], in various versions, is one of the most used questionnaires for research on CT feelings [21]. A general disadvantage of self-reports is, nevertheless, the so-called defensive bias. Therapists may find it difficult to report negative feelings. Additionally, they may not be aware of such feelings and thus unable to report them. In this paper, only the conscious affective responses are measured, and these responses are seen as a part of the total CT construct. Other researchers have focused on different CT manifestations. So far, researchers have operationalized the manifestations along behavioural, cognitive and affective levels [10].

The FWC

The instrument provides a list of different feeling words and therapists register if or to which extent these have been experienced in relation to a patient in a therapy session. The first version of the FWC was developed by Whyte et al. [15] and comprised 30 feeling words. Later, different versions have included different item numbers ranging from 24 to 58 feeling words. The different versions aimed, partly, to include feeling words which experienced therapists found lacking in the original list [12] and partly, to enhance the stability of the underlying factors in the FWC. Different factors underlying the items of the FWC have been identified, and between three and seven factors are described. Variations in the number of factors may be explained by different FWC versions and dissimilar scale formats, such as Likert scales or dichotomous yes/no versions [20].

Different statistical methods have been used to evaluate FWC. Most studies have used principal component analyses, some are based on factor analyses [22]. Furthermore, the studied samples have been heterogeneous, involving therapists of diverse professions, varying sample sizes, and different patient populations. The first studies using FWC were performed in inpatient departments. More recent studies have examined the factor structure when FWC is applied in individual therapy [12, 22,23,24]. There is still no consensus about which FWC version best captures the CT phenomenon. Generally, all studies have found at least one factor reflecting positive feelings and at least one reflecting negative feelings [22]. One goal in the CT research is to find many of the same feeling factors, as this may support that important aspects of CT have been captured. So far, many of the factors in the FWC overlap.

The present study investigates a brief FWC version (FWC-BV) featuring only 12 feeling words. A brief instrument is easier to implement in clinical contexts, thus facilitating the psychotherapy process and outcome research. An instrument such as the FWC-BV can also help therapists become more aware of their feelings. Several studies have demonstrated that therapists’ feelings are related to patient outcomes [11, 25]. For therapists to become aware of this phenomenon, it must be given attention. There is a need for research on the psychometric properties of instruments used in CT research to make future studies more robust [26]. A brief questionnaire, such as the FWC-BV, will obviously not capture all important CT feelings, but it can provide insight into meaningful aspects or patterns of feelings during therapy processes.

CT and personality disorder

There is extensive clinical and theoretical literature on CT and patients with PD, and a considerable relational strain is often reported by therapists. Particularly, many clinical articles are about borderline PD. Kernberg [27] described that these patients tend to elicit powerful CT reactions in therapists because of their intense, primitive, and regressive transferences. Furthermore, it has been argued, that specific CT feelings are the most reliable guide to diagnose borderline PD. These include the feeling of being idealised or devaluated as a therapist. There are, however, few empirical studies on PD and CT [9, 14, 28,29,30]. Two studies have explored therapists’ feelings in relation to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Version 4 (DSM-IV) diagnoses at the PD cluster level and found that that patients with cluster A (i.e., paranoid, schizoid and schizotypal PD) and B disorders (i.e., antisocial, borderline, histrionic and narcissistic PD) elicited more negative CT feelings than did patients with cluster C disorders (i.e., avoidant, dependent and obsessive compulsive PD) [9, 28]. Some studies [9, 14] have found that specific PD categories elicit different CT feelings in therapists. For example, in the study by Colli et al. [14], borderline PD was associated with emotional response feelings such as Helpless/Inadequate, Overwhelmed/Disorganised and Special/Overinvolved, while avoidant PD was associated with Parental/Protective and Special/Overinvolved responses. This study indicated that therapists’ feelings can nearly be applied diagnostically. A few studies have looked at PD dimensionally in terms of the number of fulfilled PD criteria and CT feelings. Dahl et al. [12], for example, found a strong negative relationship between the total number of fulfilled PD criteria and confident CT.

CT and therapeutic alliance (TA)

The term therapeutic alliance (TA) refers to the working relationship between the therapist and patient. To date, only a few studies have investigated the relationship between TA and CT [12, 18, 23, 26, 31]. Both the TA measurement instruments and results of these studies vary widely. Existing studies also differ in terms of whether TA is patient-rated, therapist-rated or based on both perspectives. To summarise, two studies reported a negative correlation between negative aspects of CT feelings and TA [18, 26], while others found both negative correlations between negative CT feelings and TA and positive correlations between positive CT feelings and TA [12, 23, 31].

Aims of the present study

Many former studies of CT are based on small, heterogeneous patient populations, and very few have investigated therapies with poorly functioning PD patients with PD. This study consists of a large sample of patients with significant PD pathology. In this way, it may be possible to examine the feelings that therapists can experience while working with a varied sample of PD patients.

The primary aim of the current study is to explore the factor structure and psychometric properties of the FWC-BV, used in a clinical sample of patients with PD or PD traits. Our secondary aim is to validate these factors by examining their relationship with patients’ personality pathology and TA.

More specifically, we wanted to answer the following research questions:

  1. 1.

    How many clinically meaningful factors do the items in the FWC-BV represent?

  2. 2.

    What is the relationship between therapists’ CT feelings, assessed by FWC-BV, and patients’ personality pathology?

  3. 3.

    What is the relationship between therapists’ CT feelings, assessed by FWC-BV, and patient-rated TA?

Method

Participants

Treatment units

In this multi-site, naturalistic and explorative study, data was collected from 13 outpatient units within the Norwegian Network of Personality Disorders [32] in the period 2010 to 2016. All units were outpatient services on a specialist mental health service level, providing treatment for a broad range of patients with significant personality problems and personality disorder (PD). The different units combined psychoeducational, group and individual psychotherapy formats and treatment approaches were mainly psychodynamic, but combinations also included body awareness, art and cognitive therapies. Specific PD approaches implemented within some units in the Network during the investigation period, included mentalization-based therapy, dialectical behaviour therapy and schema-focused therapy. Treatment was usually time-limited, and most units had an upper time limit between 2 and 3 years.

The different treatment units collected patient data and the therapists’ self-report questionnaires (FWC-BV), which were registered in an anonymous central database, administrated by the Department for Personality Psychiatry, Oslo University Hospital.

Therapists

In this study, all therapist data were anonymous and the number of therapists participating was unknown. However, some general information was available: The multidisciplinary therapist teams usually included psychiatrists, psychologists, psychiatric nurses and social workers. Most of the therapists are formally trained (for 3 to 5 years) in group analytic psychotherapy [32, 33]. The Network regularly provides updated courses and conferences on PD assessment procedures and therapeutic principles [32, 34]. Based on current information from 10 of the 13 units, the mean number of therapists at each unit was 10, approximately 75% female and 25% male. Mean age was 45 years. Therapists had a mean age of 17 years of clinical experience, and 73% of the therapists had education in group psychotherapy. Group supervision of the therapists is traditionally an important element in treatment programs, and CT is part of the clinical discussions. The therapists filled out the FWC-BV at 6-month intervals for each patient they had in treatment (i.e., from 6 months up to 2.5 years), with a final assessment at end of the patient’s treatment. A total of 4849 FWC-BV were completed during the study period.

Patients

The sample consisted of 2425 adult patients. They were referred to treatment within the specialist mental health service on a regular basis, from a primary health service level. The mean age was 33 years (standard deviation [SD] = 10 years), and 76% of the patients were female. According to the guidelines given in DSM-IV [35], 71% of participants had one or more PD diagnosis and 94% had at least one symptom disorder, wherein 68% had mood disorders and 57% had anxiety disorders (see Table 1 for prevalence of PDs). The severity of PD is illustrated through different outcome measures: the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF; APA, 1994) and Work and Adjustment Scale (WSAS) [36] measure patient psychosocial functioning; the Global Severity Index (GSI) measures symptom distress and is the mean score of the Revised Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90-R) [37]; and the Index of Interpersonal Problems (IIP) measures interpersonal problems and is the mean score of the Circumplex of Interpersonal Problems (CIP) [38]. The CIP is a revised version of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems – Circumplex (IIP-C) [39].

Table 1 Prevalence of PD and range of PD criteria

In the current study, the mean GAF score was 49.77 (SD = 6.06), and according to APA [35] within the “Sever” range. Mean WSAS score was 22.60 (SD = 8.56), and according to Mataix-Cols and colleagues [40] and Pedersen and colleagues [41] in the “Moderate” range. In addition, the GSI was 1.54 (SD = 0.66) and IIP was 1.65 (SD = 0.52). With respect to GSI this score is in the “Moderate” to “Sever” range [37, 42, 43], and a score of 1.65 on CIP is associated with severe interpersonal distress [42, 44]. Thus, all these measures reflect a poorly functioning patient group with a high level of symptom and interpersonal distress. TA was measured using the revised short form of the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI-SR). The patients filled out the WAI-SR at the same intervals as did the therapists when filling out the FWC-BV (i.e., every 6 months from 6 months up to 2.5 years in their treatment period, with a final assessment at the end of the treatment).

Assessment

The FWC

The FWC is a self-report measure in which therapists rate their emotional responses toward a patient in a five-point response format (0–4), ranging from ‘No such feeling’ (0) to ‘Very much’ (4). The present study uses a brief version (FWC-BV) of the Feeling Word Checklist 58 (FWC-58) that includes 12 items. In the FWC-BV, the prompt, ‘During recent conversations with the patient I have felt...’ is followed by the 12 feeling words: Disliked, Important, Threatened, Exalted, Bored, Confident, Inadequate, Admired, On Guard, Calm, Invaded and Overview. Each of the words is rated from 0 to 4 by therapists, based on how strongly they experience each feeling.

The FWC-BV is new and was constructed for this study with the aim of creating a more applicable and less time-consuming questionnaire, reflecting some positive and some negative feelings. The aim in its creation is to determine whether these positive and negative feelings are important cues to describe therapy processes and outcomes in future studies. The items were selected from the FWC-58. The selection was data-driven based on former factor analysis of the FWC-58 from a large heterogeneous material comprising data from different in- and outpatient clinics [12, 20]. We selected the 12 items with the strongest loadings in the factor structure of the FWC-58; six items with positive feelings and six items with negative feelings. The 12 items were discussed and evaluated by an experienced researcher and clinician for clinical relevance in the present study.

Diagnostics

All patients were diagnosed according to DSM-IV [35] using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) [45] for symptom disorders and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV – Axis II (SCID-II) for PD. [46] Diagnostic reliability was not investigated. However, diagnostic assessments were performed in each unit by clinical staff who had received systematic training in diagnostic interviews and principles of the Longitudinal, Expert, All-Data (LEAD) procedure [47, 48]. This means that diagnoses were based on all available information, including referral letters, self-reported history, complaints, overall clinical impression and the results of the two diagnostic interviews (i.e., the MINI and SCID-II). In DSM-IV, the classification of PD is polythetic—that is, the criteria within each disorder are neither necessary nor sufficient. The number of fulfilled PD criteria can thus be seen as a reflection of the dimensional strength or closeness to prototypic PD constructs.

TA

The patients filled in the WAI-SR [49, 50] every 6 months during treatment and at discharge from treatment. The WAI-SR is a 12-item questionnaire representing 3 different aspects of the patient’s relationship to the therapist; bond, task and goal. Patients are asked to judge each question on a Likert scale from ‘Never’ (1) to ‘Always’ (7). The patients filled out two versions of the WAI-SR: one with reference to their group therapist (WAI-G) and one with reference to their individual therapist (WAI-I).

Statistics

Unbalanced data

The data of the current study are based on ordinary routine assessments, but it is important to note that these routines sometimes are disturbed for one reason or another. Therapists may sometimes fail to fill in FWC-BV according to the time schedule, and administrative routines may be hampered so that patients do not receive six-month questionnaires. As such, the dataset in the current study is unbalanced. See Table 2 for an assessment of the FWC-BV and WAI-SR. To check for possible patient-therapist bias, patients who were evaluated on FWC-BV were compared to those patients not evaluated on FWC-BV at the time of 12 months of therapy. No significant differences were found on GAF, WSAS, GSI, CIP, or the number of fulfilled PD criteria. Thus, we found no indication of systematic bias threatening to the validity of the study results.

Table 2 FWC-BV assessments at different times

Factor analysis

We decided to analyse the FWC-BV after 12 months in therapy, assuming that therapy is well underway by that point. There is usually also some delay from the initial assessment period to inclusion in the treatment programme, although most patients have individual clinical contact with the unit during this waiting time. As such, there is good reason to assume that the treatment process is stabilised 1 year after the initial assessment.

The total sample of 2425 patients was first randomly divided into 2 separate sub-samples. This was done to facilitate the exploratory (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The first sub-sample (n = 1219) was used to conduct explorative factor analyses and the second (n = 1206) to cross-validate the suggested factor structure in a confirmatory factor analysis. After 1 year of therapy, the number of available completed FWC-BV questionnaires was 869. With respect to the initial factor analysis, sub-sample 1 (for EFA) comprised 439 FWC-BV questionnaires and sub-sample 2 (for CFA) comprised 430 FWC-BV questionnaires. All other analyses are based on the total sample of 2425 patients.

Using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25 (2017), randomisation of the total sample was done with the Select function (approximate 50%). Relationships between variables were estimated by Pearson Product-Moment Correlation, and scale reliability was estimated by McDonald’s Omega (ωt) [51, 52]. EFA and CFA were conducted using Mplus 7.11 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA) [53], with estimations based on the maximum likelihood (ML) and maximum likelihood mean (MLM) adjusted functions, respectively. The mean-adjusted chi-square test statistic, also referred to as the Satorra–Bentler chi-square [54] is robust to non-normality.

To evaluate the CFA models, goodness of fit was estimated by root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) [55], the non-normed fit index (NFI) [56]—also called the Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) [57]—the comparative fit index (CFI) [58] and the standardised root mean square residual (SRMR) [59].

An RMSEA of 0.05 or below indicates a good model fit, values between 0.05 and 0.08 indicate a reasonable fit, values between 0.08 and 0.10 indicate a mediocre fit and values above 0.10 indicate an unacceptable fit [60]. However, a cut-off value close to 0.06 [59] or a stringent upper limit of 0.07 [61] seem to be the general consensus of what is considered acceptable. The TLI and CFI both measure model fit in comparison to the independence model. Both are derived from the chi-square statistic and are supposed to lie between 0 and 1. Values greater than 0.90 for these measures are normally required for good fit of a model, although Hu and Bentler [59] have suggested TLI ≥ 0.95 as the threshold. The SRMR is the mean absolute value of the covariance residuals, and it ranges from 0 to 1. Well-fitting models should obtain values less than 0.05 [62, 63], but values up to 0.08 are acceptable [59].

Results

Factor analyses

Factor structure of the FWC-BV

The EFA of sub-sample 1 (n = 439 at 1 year) indicated four factors with eigenvalues above 1.0, accounting for 67% of the observed variance. The fifth factor had an eigenvalue of 0.716, accounting for 6% of the remaining observed variance. The CFA of sub-sample 2 (n = 430 at 1 year), based on the suggested four-factor model from the EFA, had a chi-square model fit of 99.533 (degrees of freedom [df]: 48; p = 0.0000) and an RMSEA (90% confidence interval [CI]), CFI, TLI and SRMR of 0.050 (0.036–0.064), 0.948, 0.929 and 0.054, respectively, indicating perfect fit. A subsequent CFA based on the sub-samples 1 and 2 combined (n = 869 at 1 year) also revealed good model fit. However, factor three comprised only two items (Bored and Inadequate), and Bored had a high degree of residual variance (86%). Moreover, one item of factor two (Threatened) was distinguished by a considerable lack of variance (mean: 0.05; SD: 0.28; Skewness: 6.24; Kurtosis: 45.46)—that is, hardly a strongly expressed feeling. Based on these findings, Bored and Threatened were omitted from the item pool, and Inadequate was moved to factor two. The solution was then a three-factor solution based on 10 items/feelings, labelled Idealised, Inadequate and Confident. See Table 3 for the final operationalisation of the three-factor model and estimates of scale reliabilities. As shown in Table 3, the scale reliabilities are in the acceptable range (i.e., mainly between .70 and .80) except for the factor Inadequate.

Table 3 Three-factor model of FWC-BV assessed 1 year after initial assessment and estimates of scale reliability at each assessment point

From the CFA of the new three-factor model, it was reasonable to conduct two modifications. The first was to accept a negative cross-loading from the item Important to the factor Inadequate, and the second was to accept a negative residual covariance between the item Inadequate with the item Overview of the factor Confident. In Table 4, model fit indices are shown for all assessment periods from 6 months to end of treatment. From this, all fit indices indicate excellent to good fit to the model, except from FWC evaluations at 2.5 years. The main reason for this indication of misfit was a cross-loading of the item Invaded with the factor Idealised. By accepting this cross-loading in a modified specification, the model fit was found to be acceptable (RMSEA = 0.074; CFI = 0.937; TLI = 0.902). Based on these considerations, we concluded that this three-factor structure based on 10 items revealed the best model.

Table 4 Goodness-of-fit statistics from CFA based on the three-factor model of FWC-BV questionnaires at different time points

Validity

Table 5 shows the mean levels of the three FWC-BV scales across gender and selected PDs. The mean values after 1 year of therapy was highest for Confident (mean = 2.74; SD = 0.74), followed by Idealised (mean = 1.12; SD = 0.78) and Inadequate (mean = 0.47; SD = 0.50).

Table 5 Descriptive statistics of FWC-BV subscales

To further validate the CT factors, we explored the relationship between the factors, the number of PD criteria, and TA. As shown in Table 6, Confident correlated negatively with the total number of PD criteria. Inadequate had a positive correlation with total number of PD criteria, and the borderline, narcissistic and paranoid PD criteria. Idealised showed a positive correlation with borderline and histrionic PD criteria. The avoidant PD criteria showed a weak, but not significant, positive correlation with Confident.

Table 6 Correlations between the FWC-BV and other clinical measures

Patient-rated TA showed a positive correlation with Confident and a negative correlation with Inadequate. Idealised had a positive correlation with evaluation of the individual therapists (WAI-I) but not with evaluation of the group therapists (WAI-G).

Discussion

The main objective of the current study was to investigate the factor structure and psychometric properties of the FWC-BV. We found 10 items that constituted three CT factors, namely, Idealised, Inadequate and Confident. The factors were psychometrically acceptable and clinically recognisable and can be seen as aspects of CT feelings that can be evoked when treating PD patients.

There are conceptual similarities between the present factors and several of those found in other studies. Holmquist et al. [19] were the first to examine the underlying factor structure of FWC applied in individual psychotherapy. They used an FWC with 48 feeling words (FWC-48) and found four factors to be evoked: Positive, Negative, Distant and Dejected. The factor Positive seems to show similarities with the factor Confident in our study. Negative and Dejected seem to show similarities with Inadequate. Dahl et al. [12] used a version with 58 feeling words (FWC-58) and found four factors; two of these (Confident and Inadequate) conceptually overlap with our study. The same holds for the factors Confident and Inadequate in the study by Ulberg et al. [23] (FWC-24), and the factor Inadequate in that of Lindquist et al. [22] (FWC-24). Further, in studies where the setting is inpatient care, Røssberg et al. [20] found seven factors, two of which (Inadequate and Confident) conceptually overlap with ours (FWC-58). However, we cannot say that the factors mentioned above are directly comparable, as different studies have used different FWC questionnaires [23] and included different patient populations. Idealised is a factor not reported in previous studies on FWC, but similarities between some of the previous factors reported in other studies, such as Important in the study by Røssberg et al. [20], can be identified. Nevertheless, few studies on FWC seem to have captured this aspect of CT. For an overview of previous FWC studies, see Lindquist et al. [22].

It is noteworthy that the therapists overall score a low intensity of their CT feelings. The mean scores range from 0.47 (Inadequate) to 1.12 (Idealised) to 2.74 (Confident). The scores are in a similar range to those seen in many other FWC studies — that is, quite low scores are consistently reported [12, 19, 22, 23]. However, as the patients in the present study are mainly poorly functioning PD patients, one might have expected stronger feelings to be reported. It is also somewhat surprising that Confident is the feeling assigned the highest score. This could be due to the therapists overall being highly experienced and the fact that regular group supervision is part of the therapists’ work, in which CT is a focus. This is in line with the report of Ulberg et al. [23], in which Confident was positively associated with more experience as well as with an increased level of supervision — that is, they found lower levels of Inadequate feelings with more supervision.

Alternatively, the result could also be due to defensive processes in therapists. Some may find it difficult to report negative feelings. The therapist also might not be aware of more negative feelings. As mentioned earlier, CT is partly an unconscious phenomenon, at least initially, and sometimes we only become aware of it through enactments. This is also why treatments of severe personality disorders are in greater danger of failing. However, one could expect that therapists in this study are more aware of their CT feelings as they often have supervision on it. Another explanation might be that the questionnaires were only filled in every 6 months. Some therapists have reported that ‘overall’ they feel relatively confident in meeting with the patient when they look at the relationship over a long period of time. More frequent measurements could likely capture more varied and possibly more intense CT feelings.

The three factors, Idealised, Inadequate and Confident, are consistent with aspects of feeling responses identified in clinical literature on psychotherapy with PD patients [64, 65] and in the existing empirical literature [9, 14]. Interestingly, we specifically found that in therapies with patients meeting many borderline PD criteria — therapists felt both more idealised and more inadequate. We also found that the total number of PD criteria correlated positively with the Inadequate factor. This is in line with the report of Dahl et al. [12].

In this study, feelings evoked in therapies with patients with avoidant PD were not clearly identified in the FWC-BV. Avoidant PD and borderline PD constitute the two largest patient groups in this material. Avoidant PD criteria showed a weak, but not significant, positive correlation with a Confident response. Previous empirical studies have reported that Confident is the most significant response from the group in question [21]. However, this result might also reflect that Avoidant PD patients induce greater variation in what therapist feel when treating these patients. From a clinical perspective, there is reason to believe that therapists may experience more negative feelings than previous studies have reported, especially toward more poorly functioning avoidant PD patients.

Another explanation might be that the CT response had more to do with the severity of personality pathology in terms of the number of PD criteria across specific disorders. Some researchers have questioned the validity of PDs as categorical constructs, as high PD co-occurrence exist. Sharp et al. [66] evaluated a bi-factor model for PD pathology in which a general factor and several specific factors of personality pathology account for the covariance among PD criteria. In particular, they studied the borderline PD criteria and found that they loaded only on the general factor which may suggest that BPD criteria represent core features of PD severity. Thus the nine BPD criteria may represent higher levels of disturbed behaviour. Whether the CT responses toward PD patients can be better explained by general factors is an interesting question, but beyond the scope of this paper.

Correlational analyses with patient-rated TA also revealed several meaningful and significant associations. Patient-rated TA, measured using the WAI-SR, showed a positive correlation with Confident and a negative correlation with Inadequate. This is in line with the study by Dahl et al. [12], although they rated patients’ TA with a different instrument, called the Help and Understanding Scale (HUS). The correlations between patient-rated TA and CT are of particular interest because of the non-overlapping perspectives. Idealised correlated positively with patients’ evaluation of their individual therapist (WAI-I) but not with their group therapist (WAI-G). From a clinical experience, it could be speculated that the individual therapist is idealised more than the group therapist. In treatment programs involving group therapy, patients must share a therapist’s attention with up to seven other group members, making way for more complicated feelings such as envy, feelings of exclusion and feelings of being alone among others; as such, the therapist’s lack of omnipotence is more striking than it is in individual sessions. As far as is known, no other empirical studies have found this association.

Strengths and limitations

A considerable strength of this study is that the sample was large enough to be divided in two, for EFA and subsequent CFA, and that each of the sub-samples were large enough to yield stability in the estimates. Another strength is that the data comprise a large and representative sample of patients with PD and PD traits, wherein PD not otherwise specified (NOS), borderline PD and avoidant PD are the most prevalent diagnoses. The patient sample is also well described, representing a functionally impaired and highly symptomatic patient group —a group known to evoke powerful CT reactions in therapists. No previous studies on PD and CT have investigated CT using such a large sample of poorly functioning patients. This study can thus contribute to highlight feelings that are typically described in clinical literature but only to a small extent, empirically investigated. However, the poorly functioning patients in this study may also restrict the generalisation of the results to more well-functioning, clinical samples.

One of the main limitations is the lack of information of the number of therapists participating in the study. Hence, we do not know how many patients each therapist may have treated or if the same therapist has scored the FWC for the same patient at all assessment times (i.e. every 6 months for the same patient). Thus, there is some interdependence in the data. A strength is that data from 13 different treatment units in Norway were collected, and there is reason to believe that the number of therapists is relatively high due to the number of units assessed. We also know that the staff working in the various units are relatively stable, that is, they have typically worked for several years within the same units.

Implications

This study has important clinical and research applications. As far as known, no brief version of CT questionnaires exists in the field. A brief version is easy to introduce in clinical contexts and also has several research advantages. It is easier to make repeated measurements of therapists feelings, e.g. after every session with a patient. Thus, this instrument can give a better understanding of how CT contributes in the process and outcome of therapy.

PD patients often trigger problematic countertransference reactions in therapists - and the risk of acting out in the course of treatment is higher [27]. Hence, focus on CT is particularly relevant with these patients. In this study, comprising units specialized in the treatment of PDs a focus on CT is already part of the daily work, and systematic supervision is recommended. One might assume that the level of CT feelings would be more intense or problematic in other sections of the public health system, and hence, the importance of becoming aware of CT manifestations is even higher in other areas of the health services. A brief questionnaire like FWC-BV is a short and easy instrument that can help clinicians across theoretical orientations to become more aware of their feelings during the course of treating patients.

There is a growing literature that underscores the importance of building a good working relationship with the patients [67,68,69]. Research findings demonstrate that alliance rupture repair is associated with positive psychotherapy outcome [68]. A brief instrument to measure CT feelings together with other process variables such as alliance can possibly give a better understanding of rupture-repair sequences in the therapeutic relationship. How do negative feelings affect the therapist and treatment outcome? If negative feelings are not taken seriously or are acted out they may affect the commitment to the patient, and the willingness to recognize and repair alliance ruptures. These are clinically interesting questions that should be addressed in future studies.

Conclusion

In this study, we found that the FWC-BV comprised three factors based on ten of the twelve items, labelled Inadequate, Idealised and Confident. The subscales had satisfactory internal consistency and were meaningfully related to patients’ personality pathology and TA. Thus, this shorter list of feeling words seems to identify common experiences evoked in meetings with patients with PD and may prove valuable for further research, as well as for use in clinical and educational contexts.

Availability of data and materials

Due to restrictions imposed by the Regional Medical Ethics Committee regarding patient confidentiality, data are available upon request. Requests for data may be sent to the hospital’s Privacy and Data Protection Officer at: personvern@ous-hf.no

Change history

  • 27 August 2021

    The incorrect peer review reports were published in the original publication. The article has been updated to rectify the errors.

Abbreviations

CFA:

Confirmatory factor analysis

CFI:

Comparative fit index

CIP:

Circumplex of interpersonal problems

CT:

Countertransference

df:

Degree of freedom

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders version 4

EFA:

Exploratory factor analysis

FWC-BV:

Feeling Word Checklist brief version

FWC:

Feeling Word Checklist

GAF:

Global Assessment of Functioning

GSI:

Global Severity Index

HUS:

Help and Understanding Scale

IIP:

Index of Interpersonal Problems

LEAD:

Longitudinal, Expert, All-Data

MIIC:

Mean Inter-Item Correlation

M.I.N.I.:

Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview

ML:

Maximum likelihood

MLM:

Maximum Likelihood Mean Adjusted

NFI:

Non-normed fit index

ωt :

Mc Donald’s coefficient Omega

PD:

Personality disorder

PD NOS:

Personality disorder not otherwise specified

RMSEA:

Root mean square error of approximation

SCID-II:

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV – Axis II Personality Disorders

SCL-90-R:

Revised Symptom Checklist

SD:

Standard deviation

SRMR:

Standardised root mean square residual

TLI:

Tucker Lewis Index

WAI-G:

Working Alliance Inventory – Group Therapist

TA:

Therapeutic alliance

WAI-SR:

Short version of Working Alliance Inventory

WAI-I:

Working Alliance Inventory – Individual Therapist

WSAS:

Work and Adjustment Scale

References

  1. Freud S, Strachey J, Freud A. The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud Vol. 11 (1910): five lectures on psycho-analysis, Leonardo da Vinci, and other works. London: Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis; 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Winnicott D. Hate in the counter-transference. Int J Psychoanal. 1949;30:69.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Heimann P. On counter-transference. Int J Psychoanal. 1950;31:81.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Aron L. A meeting of minds. Hillsdale: The Analytic Press; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Greenberg J. The analyst’s participation: a new look. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. 2001;49(2):359–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Ogden TH. Subjects of analysis: Jason Aronson, Incorporated; 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Gabbard GO. Psychodynamic psychiatry in clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2014.

  8. Gabbard GO. Narcissism and its dicontents: diagnostic dilemmas and treatment strategies with narcissistic patients. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2018.

  9. Betan E, Heim AK, Zittel Conklin C, Westen D. Countertransference phenomena and personality pathology in clinical practice: an empirical investigation. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(5):890–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Fauth J. Toward more (and better) countertransference research. Psychotherapy. 2006;43(1):1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hayes JA, Gelso CJ, Goldberg S, Kivlighan DM. Countertransference management and effective psychotherapy: meta-analytic findings. Psychotherapy. 2018;55(4):496–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Dahl HS, Rossberg JI, Bogwald KP, Gabbard GO, Hoglend PA. Countertransference feelings in one year of individual therapy: an evaluation of the factor structure in the Feeling Word Checklist-58. Psychother Res. 2012;22(1):12–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Tanzilli A, Colli A, Del Corno F, Lingiardi V. Factor structure, reliability, and validity of the therapist response questionnaire. Personal Disord. 2016;7(2):147–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Colli A, Tanzilli A, Dimaggio G, Lingiardi V. Patient personality and therapist response: an empirical investigation. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(1):102–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Whyte CR, Constantopoulos C, Bevans HG. Types of countertransference identified by Q-analysis. Br J Clin Psychol. 1982;55(2):187–201.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Loffler-Stastka H, Sell C, Zimmermann J, Huber D, Klug G. Is countertransference a valid source of clinical information? Investigating emotional responses to audiotaped psychotherapy sessions. Bull Menn Clin. 2019;83(4):353–75.

  17. Rosenberger EW, Hayes JA. Origins, consequences, and management of countertransference: a case study. J Couns Psychol. 2002;49(2):221–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Ligiéro DP, Gelso CJ. Countertransference, attachment, and the working alliance: the therapist's contribution. Psychotherapy. 2002;39(1):3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Holmqvist R, Hansjons-Gustafsson U, Gustafsson J. Patients’ relationship episodes and therapists’ feelings. Psychol Psychother. 2002;75(4):393–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rossberg JI, Hoffart A, Friis S. Psychiatric staff members’ emotional reactions toward patients. A psychometric evaluation of an extended version of the Feeling Word Checklist (FWC-58). Nordic J Psychiatry. 2003;57(1):45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Colli A, Ferri M. Patient personality and therapist countertransference. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2015;28(1):46–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lindqvist K, Falkenstrom F, Sandell R, Holmqvist R, Ekeblad A, Thoren A. Multilevel exploratory factor analysis of the Feeling Word Checklist-24. Assessment. 2017;24(7):907–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ulberg R, Falkenberg A, Naerdal T, Johannessen H, Olsen J, Eide T, et al. Countertransference feelings when treating teenagers. A psychometric evaluation of the Feeling Word Checklist-24. Am J Psychother. 2013;67(4):347–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Daniel SIF, Lunn S, Poulsen S. Client attachment and therapist feelings in the treatment of bulimia nervosa. Psychotherapy. 2015;52(2):247–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Machado Dde B, Coelho FM, Giacomelli AD, Donassolo MA, Abitante MS, Dall'Agnol T, et al. Systematic review of studies about countertransference in adult psychotherapy. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2014;36(4):173–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Machado DB, Teche SP, Lapolli C, Tavares BF, Almeida LSP, Silva GBD, et al. Countertransference and therapeutic alliance in the early stage of adult psychodynamic psychotherapy. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2015;37(3):133–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kernberg OF. Psychodynamic psychotherapy of borderline patients. New York: Basic Books; 1989.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Rossberg JI, Karterud S, Pedersen G, Friis S. An empirical study of countertransference reactions toward patients with personality disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2007;48(3):225–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Gazzillo F, Lingiardi V, Del Corno F, Genova F, Bornstein RF, Gordon RM, et al. Clinicians’ emotional responses and psychodynamic diagnostic manual adult personality disorders: a clinically relevant empirical investigation. Psychotherapy. 2015;52(2):238–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Tanzilli A, Lingiardi V, Hilsenroth M. Patient SWAP-200 personality dimensions and FFM traits: do they predict therapist responses? Personal Disord. 2018;9(3):250–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Najavits LM, Griffin ML, Luborsky L, Frank A, Weiss RD, Liese BS, et al. Therapists’ emotional reactions to substance abusers: a new questionnaire and initial findings. Psychotherapy. 1995;32(4):669–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Karterud S, Pedersen G, Bjordal E, Brabrand J, Friis S, Haaseth O, et al. Day treatment of patients with personality disorders: experiences from a Norwegian treatment research network. J Personal Disord. 2003;17(3):243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Karterud S. Gruppeanalyse og psykodynamisk gruppepsykoterapi. Oslo: Pax; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Karterud S, Urnes Ø, Pedersen G. Personlighetsforstyrrelser: forståelse, evaluering, kombinert gruppebehandling. Oslo: Pax; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  35. DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. APA; 1994.

  36. Mundt JC, Marks IM, Shear MK, Greist JM. The work and social adjustment scale: a simple measure of impairment in functioning. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;180:461–4.

  37. Derogatis LR. Symptom checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R): administration, scoring and procedures manual. Minneapolis: National Computer System; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Alden LE, Wiggins JS, Pincus AL. Construction of circumplex scales for the inventory of interpersonal problems. J Pers Assess. 1990;55(3–4):521–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Pedersen GA. Norsk revidert versjon av inventory of interpersonal problems—circumplex (IIP-C) [revised Norwegian version of the inventory of interpersonal problems—circumplex (IIP-C)]. Tidsskrift for Norsk Psykologforening. 2002;39(1):25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Mataix-Cols D, Cowley AJ, Hankins M, Schneider A, Bachofen M, Kenwright M, et al. Reliability and validity of the work and social adjustment scale in phobic disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2005;46(3):223–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Pedersen G, Kvarstein EH, Wilberg T. The work and social adjustment scale: psychometric properties and validity among males and females, and outpatients with and without personality disorders. Personal Ment Health. 2017;11(4):215–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Pedersen G. Assessment of self-reported symptoms and interpersonal problems: psychometric considerations and validity of clinical use. London: Warnborough University; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Vassend O, Lian L, Andresen HT. Norske versjoner av NEO-personality inventory, symptom checklist 90 revised og Giessen subjective complaints list. Del I Tidsskrift for Norsk Psykologforening. 1992;29:1150–60.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Kvarstein EH, Pedersen G, Folmo E, Urnes O, Johansen MS, Hummelen B, et al. Mentalization-based treatment or psychodynamic treatment programmes for patients with borderline personality disorder - the impact of clinical severity. Psychol Psychother. 2019;92(1):91–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Sheehan D, Lecrubier Y, Janavs J. Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI). Tampa: University of South Florida, Institute for Research in Psychiatry; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  46. First MB. User's guide for the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis II personality disorders: SCID-II. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Spitzer RL. Psychiatric diagnosis: are clinicians still necessary? Compr Psychiatry. 1983;24(5):399–411.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pedersen G, Karterud S, Hummelen B, Wilberg T. The impact of extended longitudinal observation on the assessment of personality disorders. Personal Ment Health. 2013;7(4):277–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Horvath AO, Greenberg LS. Development and validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. J Couns Psychol. 1989;36(2):223–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hatcher RL, Gillaspy JA. Development and validation of a revised short version of the Working Alliance Inventory. Psychother Res. 2006;16(1):12–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. McDonald RP. Test theory : a unified treatment. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Trizano-Hermosilla I, Alvarado JM. Best alternatives to Cronbach’s alpha reliability in realistic conditions: congeneric and asymmetrical measurements. Front Psychol. 2016;7:769.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user's guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles: Muthèn & Muthèn; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Satorra A, Bentler P. A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika. 2001;66(4):507–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Steiger JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: an interval estimation approach. Multivar Behav Res. 1990;25(2):173–80.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Bentler PM, Bonett DG. Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol Bull. 1980;88(3):588–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Tucker LR, Lewis C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1973;38(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107(2):238–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods. 1996;1(2):130–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Steiger JH. Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Pers Individ Differ. 2007;42(5):893–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with EQS: basic concepts, applications, and programming. 2nd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  63. Diamantopoulos A. Introducing LISREL: a guide for the unintiated. London: SAGE Publications Ltd.; 2000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  64. Gabbard GO. The treatment of the ‘special’ patient in a psychoanalytic hospital. Int Rev Psychoanal. 1986;13(3):333–47.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Klein M. Notes on some schizoid mechanisms. Int J Psychoanal. 1996;5(2):160.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Sharp C, Wright AGC, Fowler JC, Frueh BC, Allen JG, Oldham J, et al. The structure of personality pathology: both general (‘g’) and specific (‘s’) factors? J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124(2):387–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Flückiger C, Del Re AC, Wampold BE, Horvath AO. The Alliance in adult psychotherapy: a meta-analytic synthesis. Psychotherapy. 2018;55(4):316–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Eubanks CF, Muran JC, Safran JD. Alliance rupture repair: a meta-analysis. Psychotherapy. 2018;55(4):508–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Safran JD, Muran JC, Eubanks-Carter C. Repairing Alliance ruptures. Psychotherapy. 2011;48(1):80–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the patients and staff of the Norwegian Network of Personality-Disorder for their contribution to this study.

Funding

No funding was obtained for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RB: data analysis and drafting and revising the manuscript; TW: revising the manuscript; JE: revising the manuscript; JIR: revising the manuscript; HSD: revising the manuscript; GP: data analysis and drafting and revising the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to R. Breivik.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All patients gave written, informed consent to participate in the research. The different treatment units collected patient data, which were registered in an anonymous central database, administrated by the Department for Personality Psychiatry, Oslo University Hospital. The State Data Inspectorate and the Regional Committee for Medical Research and Ethics approved these procedures based on the anonymity of the data.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Breivik, R., Wilberg, T., Evensen, J. et al. Countertransference feelings and personality disorders: a psychometric evaluation of a brief version of the Feeling Word Checklist (FWC-BV). BMC Psychiatry 20, 141 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02556-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02556-6

Keywords