Review
Reinforcement sensitivity, depression and anxiety: A meta-analysis and meta-analytic structural equation model

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101842Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Sensitivity to punishment positively predicts both depression and anxiety.

  • Sensitivity to reward discriminates between them, negatively predicting depression.

  • This pattern was observed even when directly controlling for comorbidity.

  • Depression’s effect sizes are uniquely sensitive to clinical state.

  • Depression’s effect sizes are also moderated by method of clinical assessment.

Abstract

Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) posits that individual differences in reward and punishment processing predict differences in cognition, behavior, and psychopathology. We performed a quantitative review of the relationships between reinforcement sensitivity, depression and anxiety, in two separate sets of analyses. First, we reviewed 204 studies that reported either correlations between reinforcement sensitivity and self-reported symptom severity or differences in reinforcement sensitivity between diagnosed and healthy participants, yielding 483 effect sizes. Both depression (Hedges’ g = .99) and anxiety (g = 1.21) were found to be high on punishment sensitivity. Reward sensitivity negatively predicted only depressive disorders (g = −.21). More severe clinical states (e.g., acute vs remission) predicted larger effect sizes for depression but not anxiety. Next, we reviewed an additional 39 studies that reported correlations between reinforcement sensitivity and both depression and anxiety, yielding 156 effect sizes. We then performed meta-analytic structural equation modeling to simultaneously estimate all covariances and control for comorbidity. Again we found punishment sensitivity to predict depression (β = .37) and anxiety (β = .35), with reward sensitivity only predicting depression (β = −.07). The transdiagnostic role of punishment sensitivity and the discriminatory role of reward sensitivity support a hierarchical approach to RST and psychopathology.

Introduction

Gray’s Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST; Gray, 1970, Gray, 1987; Gray & McNaughton, 2000) posits that sensitivity to appetitive and aversive stimuli serves as a biological basis of human personality. According to the original theory, the Behavioral Approach System (BAS) governs processes related to appetitive stimuli, whereas the separate, independent Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) governs responses to aversive stimuli (Corr, 2008). Individuals’ varied sensitivities in BAS and BIS functioning then lead to individual differences in reward processing, punishment processing, and personality. Those with a more sensitive BAS tend to show more cognitive styles and behaviors associated with reward promotion and the broad personality dimension of extraversion (Corr & McNaughton, 2008; Depue & Collins, 1999; Gray, 1987). A more sensitive BIS, on the other hand, impacts behaviors and psychological processes related to punishment and neuroticism (Gray, 1970; Smits & Boeck, 2006).

In 2000, RST was revised (Gray & McNaughton, 2000) to include three systems (Corr, 2008). The BAS system remained governing all appetitive processing, while aversive processing was ascribed to the newly named Fight/Flight/Freeze System (FFFS) instead of the original BIS. The BIS in the revised version was theorized to be activated when the goals of the BAS and FFFS came into approach-avoidance conflicts. New measures (e.g., Corr & Cooper, 2016; Jackson, 2009) have been developed with the purpose of differentiating the revised BIS from the FFFS (i.e., original BIS). In practice, they both correlate with measures of BIS developed under the original RST and much of the literature on RST, motivation and psychopathology continues to use the original terminology of BAS for appetitive sensitivity and BIS for aversive sensitivity (e.g., Bijttebier, Beck, Claes, & Vandereycken, 2009; Trew, 2011). The current meta-analysis, in reviewing this literature, uses the terminology of the original RST when referring to basic appetitive and aversive processes as well.

When appetitive and aversive sensitivities are dysregulated, they are theorized to be diatheses for subsequent psychopathology. BAS hyposensitivity is indicated by deficits in anticipatory pleasure, consummatory pleasure, and appetitive motivation (Alloy, Olino, Freed, & Nusslock, 2016; Olino, McMakin, & Forbes, 2018; Zald & Treadway, 2017). This anhedonic experience is a hallmark of dysthymia and major depressive disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Watson et al., 1995). Accordingly, numerous studies have found reduced BAS sensitivity to be associated with depression among clinical (e.g., DelDonno et al., 2015; Greenwood et al., 2013; Zaninotto et al., 2015) and nonclinical samples (e.g., Hundt, Nelson-Gray, Kimbrel, Mitchell, & Kwapil, 2007; Jarmolowicz et al., 2014; Peirson & Heuchert, 2001) – though not always (e.g., Carver & Johnson, 2009). Among depressed patients, BAS hyposensitivity was found to predict greater concurrent symptom severity and a worse outcome following treatment (Kasch, Rottenberg, Arnow, & Gotlib, 2002). Anxiety, on the other hand, is theorized to be weakly related to BAS sensitivity, if at all (Gray, 1987; Gray & McNaughton, 2000; McNaughton & Corr, 2004). Rather, anxiety is theorized to primarily occur as a function of aversive sensitivities, conceptualized as BIS hypersensitivity in the original RST (Gray, 1982, Gray, 1987). BIS hypersensitivity is associated with increased apprehension in the face of impending negative outcomes and more intense reactivity to punishment, which are experiences salient to anxiety disorders (Carver & White, 1994; Corr & McNaughton, 2008; Hollon, 2019; Leen-Feldner, Zvolensky, Feldner, & Lejuez, 2004). Later investigations, however, have found BIS levels to predict depression to a large degree as well (Eshel & Roiser, 2010; Hundt et al., 2007; Johnson, Turner, & Iwata, 2003; Pekka Jylhä & Isometsä, 2006).

Thus, it appears that the BIS predicts both depression and anxiety while the BAS uniquely predicts depression (Bijttebier et al., 2009; Khan, Jacobson, Gardner, Prescott, & Kendler, 2005). This pattern has been linked to similar hierarchical models of psychopathology such as the tripartite model, where general distress is present across depression and anxiety disorders, but deficits in positive affect are unique to depression (Clark & Watson, 1991; Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman, & Zald, 2017; Zinbarg & Yoon, 2008).

The multidisciplinary literature on the relationship between reinforcement sensitivity dysregulation and mood disorders has been summarized in a number of influential theoretical frameworks and models. The BAS dysregulation model highlights the unique role that reward sensitivity plays in the development and maintenance of depression and bipolar disorder (Alloy et al., 2016; Stange et al., 2013). The joint system hypothesis suggests that internalizing pathology is most closely predicted by a confluence of hyposensitive BAS and hypersensitive BIS (Corr, 2001; Eddington, Majestic, & Silvia, 2012; Hundt et al., 2007). Furthermore, RST has provided a theoretical basis for disorder classification models related to depression and anxiety in particular (Nusslock, Abramson, Harmon-Jones, Alloy, & Hogan, 2007; Zinbarg & Yoon, 2008), and mental illness in general (Bijttebier et al., 2009). RST has even served key roles in explaining how individual differences in general may impact the etiology (Kimbrel, 2008; Trew, 2011), severity (e.g., Brown, Chorpita, & Barlow, 1998; Cloninger, Bayon, & Svrakic, 1998) and classification (e.g., Caspi et al., 2014; Clark & Watson, 1991; Eaton et al., 2013; Krueger & Markon, 2006; Watson, 2009) of mental disorders. The National Institute of Mental Health has since included reward and punishment processing among its Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; Insel et al., 2010), further emphasizing the central role that RST plays in basic clinical science.

The current theoretical work, however, is limited by its reliance on narrative reviews, which often under-represent nonsignificant effects, unpublished effects, or effects that were secondary to the study at hand (Easterbrook, Gopalan, Berlin, & Matthews, 1991; Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001; Sterne, Gavaghan, & Egger, 2000). This biased source selection may have theoretical implications. Constructs such as positive emotionality (Watson & Naragon-Gainey, 2010) and extraversion (Krueger, Caspi, Moffitt, Silva, & McGee, 1996) have been hypothesized to differentiate depression from anxiety. However, when subjected to the scrutiny of meta-analyses, neither construct conclusively discriminated between these disorders (Khazanov & Ruscio, 2016; Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010). Similarly, the relationship between BAS hyposensitivity and anhedonic depression has been highlighted in narrative reviews of findings (e.g., Alloy et al., 2016; Zinbarg & Yoon, 2008) derived from biological (e.g., DelDonno et al., 2015), behavioral (e.g., Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton, & Zald, 2012), and self-report data (e.g., Kircanski, Mazur, & Gotlib, 2013), with some studies based upon quite sizeable samples (e.g., Johnson et al., 2003). However, it is also possible that the unique relationship between BAS sensitivity and anhedonia may not withstand the scrutiny of meta-analysis, as was the case of positive emotionality and extraversion.

Attempts to discriminate between depression and anxiety are further complicated by their high comorbidity (Watson, 2009). Depression and anxiety are noted risk factors for each other (Jacobson & Newman, 2017), with overlapping symptom criteria (Borsboom & Cramer, 2013), some shared basic mechanisms (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011; Ruscio, Seitchik, Gentes, Jones, & Hallion, 2011), and are especially comorbid among more severe clinical samples (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005). To understand each disorder’s unique relationship with reinforcement sensitivity, it is necessary to control for their shared variance. Only meta-analytic tools enable that, either by indirectly projecting shared covariance (e.g., Khazanov & Ruscio, 2016) or by using structural equation modeling to directly control for it (M. W.-L. Cheung & Chan, 2005; Jak, 2015). Thus, a systematic quantitative review is needed to adequately estimate the unique relationships between BAS, BIS, depression and anxiety, after controlling for the disorders’ high rates of comorbidity.

Furthermore, a meta-analysis of the relationship between reinforcement sensitivity, depression and anxiety may answer the calls for identifying potential moderators to the relationship (see Bijttebier et al., 2009). One moderator of note is clinical state. Indeed, differences have been found between acute depressed patients and those in remission in measures of both reinforcement sensitivity (Pinto-Meza et al., 2006) and temperament (Nery et al., 2009; Takahashi et al., 2013). This was particularly true for punishment sensitivity (Hansenne & Bianchi, 2009). To the best of our knowledge, however, the moderating role of clinical state on otherwise stable traits is largely neglected in the meta-analytic literature (Kotov et al., 2010; cf. Zaninotto et al., 2016), including the literature on reinforcement sensitivity.

In sum, numerous multimodal lines of research have found that dysregulated reinforcement sensitivity plays a critical role in the development and maintenance of depression and anxiety. Summaries of these findings, however, have been limited to narrative reviews. Without a quantitative summary of the literature, the range of observed effect sizes remains unknown, central hypotheses laid out in RST remain untested, and potential moderators remain unexamined. These questions impact the nature of reinforcement sensitivity’s relationship with depression and anxiety, and warrant the performance of a meta-analysis.

Section snippets

The current studies and hypotheses

The current meta-analyses aimed to quantify the relationships between reinforcement sensitivity, depression and anxiety, and to examine the factors that may moderate these relationships. First, we examined the broad relationships between reward and punishment sensitivity, and a single pathology factor consisting of both depression and anxiety (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Caspi et al., 2014). In doing so, we aimed to provide, for the first time, an estimation of the true effect

Literature search

Studies were identified through a set of 30 searches in PsycInfo and PubMed between 1991 and October 2017. Searches entailed permutations of (a) keywords related to reinforcement sensitivity theory and its scales - RST, "Reinforcement Sensitivity", "Reward Sensitivity", "Punishment Sensitivity", "Reward Dependence", BIS, BAS,Behavioral Activation System”, “Behavioral Approach”, “Behavioral Inhibition”, SPSRQ,Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire”, “Brief Sensitivity to Punishment”, “

Data analytic plan

All effect sizes were transformed to standard mean differences using standard formulae (H. Cooper, Hedges, & Valentine, 2009). To correct for small sample sizes, Hedges’ g was used instead of Cohen’s d (Hedges & Olkin, 1984), with non-clinical participants coded as 0 and clinical participants coded as 1. Thus, positive effect sizes indicated elevated reinforcement sensitivity among those with higher clinical measures. We considered an absolute value of Hedges’ g under |.10| to be trivial,

Study 2

In order to account for high levels of covariance between depression and anxiety, Study 2 aimed to simultaneously estimate each relationship between appetitive sensitivity (i.e., BAS), aversive sensitivity (i.e., BIS), depression and anxiety. This was done using a meta-analytic structural equation modeling approach (MASEM; Hunter & Schmidt, 2004).

General discussion

The relationships between reinforcement sensitivity (Corr & McNaughton, 2008; Gray, 1970, Gray, 1987; Gray & McNaughton, 2000) depression and anxiety have been widely studied and brought together through influential theoretical narratives (e.g., Alloy et al., 2016; Kimbrel, 2008; McNaughton & Corr, 2008; Trew, 2011; Zinbarg & Yoon, 2008). However, until now, there has been no attempt to summarize the literature quantitatively or to systematically consider factors that may moderate these

Conclusion

The present meta-analyses summarize the current state of RST, depression and anxiety, and signal new avenues of future inquiry. Ultimately, we found robust support for a hierarchical approach to reinforcement sensitivity (e.g., Zinbarg & Yoon, 2008) that may be consistent with the joint subsystem hypothesis (e.g., Corr, 2002). Punishment hypersensitivity was found to be a higher-order, shared factor for both depression and anxiety, whereas reward hyposensitivity was specific to depression.

Acknowledgements

This research was partially supported by the Israel Science Foundation grant 886/18 awarded to IY. Author BAK was involved in all parts of the current research. Author KM assisted in literature searches, coding of data and editing of the manuscript. Author GA contributed to writing of the manuscript. Author IY supervised study design, analysis and writing of the manuscript. All authors have approved the final manuscript. The authors have no conflicts of interest to report. The authors thank

Benjamin A. Katz, MA, is a doctoral candidate in the Personality, Cognition and Psychopathology Laboratory in the Department of Psychology at The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

References, (407)

  • M. *Balsamo

    Personality and depression: Evidence of a possible mediating role for anger trait in the relationship between cooperativeness and depression

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • M. *Battaglia et al.

    Temperament dimensions explain the comorbidity of psychiatric disorders

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (1996)
  • U. *Berger et al.

    The behavioral inhibition system (BIS) mediates major aspects of the relationship between disgust and OCD symptomology

    Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders

    (2014)
  • O.J. Bienvenu et al.

    Normal personality traits and comorbidity among phobic, panic and major depressive disorders

    Psychiatry Research

    (2001)
  • P. Bijttebier et al.

    Gray’s Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory as a framework for research on personality-psychopathology associations

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2009)
  • C. *Booth et al.

    Social anxiety and alcohol consumption: The role of alcohol expectancies and reward sensitivity

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2009)
  • A.M. *Brailean et al.

    Attentional modulation by reward and punishment cues in relation to depressive symptoms

    Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • S.L. *Brown et al.

    The relationship of personality to mood and anxiety states: A dimensional approach

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (1992)
  • J.R. Carl et al.

    Development and preliminary evaluation of a positive emotion regulation augmentation module for anxiety and depression

    Behavior Therapy

    (2018)
  • J.R. Carl et al.

    A preliminary investigation of the effects of the unified protocol on temperament

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • J.R. Carl et al.

    Positive emotion regulation in emotional disorders: A theoretical review

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2013)
  • X. *Caseras et al.

    The measurement of individual differences in behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems: A comparison of personality scales

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2003)
  • F.C. *Celikel et al.

    Cloninger’s temperament and character dimensions of personality in patients with major depressive disorder

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • J.-S. †Choi et al.

    Dysfunctional inhibitory control and impulsivity in Internet addiction

    Psychiatry Research

    (2014)
  • L. *Claes et al.

    The relationship between compulsive buying, eating disorder symptoms, and temperament in a sample of female students

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • C.R. *Cloninger et al.

    Measurement of temperament and character in mood disorders: a model of fundamental states as personality types

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (1998)
  • A.A. *Contractor et al.

    PTSD’s underlying symptom dimensions and relations with behavioral inhibition and activation

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2013)
  • R.J. Coplan et al.

    A person-oriented analysis of behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in children

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2006)
  • P.J. Corr

    Testing problems in J. A. Gray’s personality theory

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2001)
  • P.J. Corr

    J. A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory: Tests of the joint subsystems hypothesis of anxiety and impulsivity

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2002)
  • D.S. *Cowley et al.

    Personality and benzodiazepine sensitivity in anxious patients and control subjects

    Psychiatry Research

    (1993)
  • P. Cuijpers et al.

    Behavioral activation treatments of depression: A meta-analysis

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2007)
  • E. *Dalbudak et al.

    Alexithymia and personality in relation to social anxiety among university students

    Psychiatry Research

    (2013)
  • V. *De Pascalis et al.

    Effects of personality trait emotionality on acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition including N100 and P200 event-related potential

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2013)
  • S.R. *DelDonno et al.

    Affective personality predictors of disrupted reward learning and pursuit in major depressive disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2015)
  • K. *Aarts et al.

    Anxiety not only increases, but also alters early error-monitoring functions

    Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2010)
  • M. †Ak et al.

    Temperament and character properties of male psoriasis patients

    Journal of Health Psychology

    (2012)
  • M. *Alfimova et al.

    Interaction effects of the COMT and DRD4 genes with anxiety-related traits on selective attention

    The Spanish Journal of Psychology

    (2014)
  • L.B. Alloy et al.

    The role of the behavioral approach system (BAS) in bipolar spectrum disorders

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2010)
  • L.B. Alloy et al.

    Behavioral approach system and behavioral inhibition system sensitivities and bipolar spectrum disorders: Prospective prediction of bipolar mood episodes

    Bipolar Disorders

    (2008)
  • L.B. Alloy et al.

    Longitudinal predictors of bipolar spectrum disorders: A behavioral approach system perspective

    Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice

    (2009)
  • L.B. Alloy et al.

    Progression along the bipolar spectrum: A longitudinal study of predictors of conversion from bipolar spectrum conditions to bipolar I and II disorders

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2012)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2013)
  • B.A. *Ammerman et al.

    Using propensity scores to examine the association between behavioral inhibition/activation and nonsuicidal and suicidal self-injury

    Crisis

    (2017)
  • J. †Atkinson et al.

    Behavioral activation and inhibition, negative affect, and gambling severity in a sample of young adult college students

    Journal of Gambling Studies

    (2012)
  • S. *Bajraktarov et al.

    Personality vharacteristics as predictive factors for the occurrence of depressive disorder

    Open Access Maced J Med Sci

    (2017)
  • M. Bakker et al.

    The rules of the game called psychological science

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2012)
  • D.H. Barlow et al.

    Unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: Therapist guide

    (2010)
  • D.H. Barlow et al.

    The nature, diagnosis, and treatment of neuroticism: Back to the future

    Clinical Psychological Science

    (2014)
  • M. *Battaglia et al.

    An investigation of the co-occurrence of panic and somatization disorders through temperamental variables

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (1998)
  • Cited by (0)

    Benjamin A. Katz, MA, is a doctoral candidate in the Personality, Cognition and Psychopathology Laboratory in the Department of Psychology at The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

    Kathryn Matanky, BA, is a post-baccalaureate research assistant in the Personality, Cognition and Psychopathology Laboratory in the Department of Psychology at The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

    Gidi Aviram, MA, is a doctoral candidate in the Personality, Cognition and Psychopathology Laboratory in the Department of Psychology at The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

    Iftah Yovel, PhD, is a Senior Lecturer of Psychology and the director of the Personality, Cognition and Psychopathology Laboratory in the Department of Psychology at The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

    Articles included in Study 1.

    Articles included in Study 2.

    View full text