Opinion
Embryo Selection and Mate Choice: Can ‘Honest Signals’ Be Trusted?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2019.12.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Mate choice by honest signaling is a classic explanation for elaborate traits in nature. Many researchers have: (i) observed deceptive signaling, and (ii) wondered how honest signals relate to trait elaboration.

  • Honest signaling is analogous to high-stakes testing. Quality is hard to measure directly, so proxies (tests) are used. High-stakes testing causes ‘teaching to the test’ without improving educational outcomes.

  • Embryo choice is another high-stakes test. Mothers select healthy embryos and terminate sub-par embryos automatically. Embryos are selected to pass maternal tests without improving their quality. The resulting arms race causes extreme and elaborate signals during pregnancy.

  • We can better understand elaborate traits in nature if we interpret mate selection, and embryo choice, as a dynamic give-and-take between two parties with conflicting fitness interests.

When a measure becomes a target, it often ceases to be a good measure – an effect familiar from the declining usefulness of standardized testing in schools. This economic principle also applies to mate choice and, perhaps surprisingly, pregnancy. Just as females screen potential mates under many metrics, human mothers unconsciously screen embryos for quality. ‘Examinees’ are under intense selection to improve test performance by exaggerating formerly ‘honest’ signals of quality. Examiners must change their screening criteria to maintain useful information (but cannot abandon old criteria unilaterally). By the resulting ‘proxy treadmill’, new honest indicators arise while old degraded indicators linger, resulting in trait elaboration and exaggeration. Hormone signals during pregnancy show extreme evolutionary escalation (akin to elaborate mating displays).

Section snippets

If the Stakes Are High, Measurement Is Hard

High-stakes testing is meant to measure performance in many contexts, but it also causes systemic, and unwanted behavior change. Often the things tested are easily measured proxies (e.g., standardized test scores at a middle school) for more difficult-to-measure attributes (e.g., how well the middle school teachers educate their pupils). Teachers ‘teach to the test’ rather than improving students’ reading skills [1]. Poorly performing students are sometimes encouraged to stay home [2,3]. In a

Choosing a Partner: The Displaying Sex Has Incentives to ‘Cheat’, but Some Forces Resist

In mate choice, members of one sex, usually females, assess the performance of the other sex in a mating display. Multiple selective pressures interact to shape female preference and favor particular male traits, including sensory bias (pre-existing preference for particular traits [14,15]), arbitrary aesthetic preferences [16., 17., 18., 19., 20.], and the need to identify members of one’s own species [21]; however, the most common explanation is that females use display traits to select a

The Proxy Treadmill: New Honest Signals Evolve and Degraded Signals Linger

Observable proxies are chosen because they correlate with more difficult-to-observe qualities. However, this correlation tends to weaken because the proxy becomes a direct target of selection for test-taking ability. This can be thought of as ‘sensory manipulation’ of the examiner by the examinee. If examiners are to continue to select higher-quality examinees, they must seek new proxies that are better correlates of desired qualities. However, there is a conservative force, recognized by

Choosing a Child: Signal Elaboration by the Proxy Treadmill during Human Pregnancy

One rationale for ‘honest signaling’ or ‘good genes’ models of mate choice is that females select males based on the expected quality of their genetic contribution to offspring. This is indirect selection of offspring: a high-quality male may father some low-quality offspring because of the vagaries of genetic recombination and epistatic interactions between maternal and paternal genomes. A more direct way to improve the quality of offspring would be to examine offspring themselves before major

Concluding Remarks: Why Are Placentas Like Birds-of-Paradise?

Placentas must achieve a simple task: passing goods between mother and fetus. It should be a piece of cake. But placentas are wildly diverse across mammals, ‘evolving and experimenting at a disturbingly rapid pace’ [47], reminiscent of the bizarre diversity of birds-of-paradise [64]. Pregnancy in placental mammals is a case of antagonistic coevolution between signaler and receiver. The sensory onslaughts of placental hormones in embryo choice, and fantastic ornaments in mate choice, are

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to two anonymous reviewers for their feedback. We would like to thank John Christy for particularly useful feedback, including suggesting the terminology ‘aesthetic residue’ and suggesting multiple specific predictions and outstanding questions. We are grateful to Arvid Ågren, Pavitra Muralidhar, Carl Veller, and Inbar Mayaan for comments on the manuscript. D.E.M.’s research is conducted with Government support under and awarded by DoD, Air Force Office of Scientific

Glossary

Aesthetic residue
display trait which is no longer indicative of quality but is nonetheless retained in signaler–displayer examinations because it would be costly to abandon.
Campbell’s law
a principle in social sciences that states that quantitative proxies, or metrics, meant to measure complicated phenomena are subject to corruption when the indicator is used for decision-making. ‘When a measure becomes a target it ceases to be a good measure.’
Chorionic gonadotropin (CG)
hormone produced by the

References (91)

  • L.A. Cole

    HCG, five independent molecules

    Clin. Chim. Acta

    (2012)
  • P.L. Smith

    Equine lutropin and chorionic gonadotropin bear oligosaccharides terminating with SO4-4-GalNAc and Sia alpha 2, 3Gal, respectively

    J. Biol. Chem.

    (1993)
  • A. Henke et al.

    New insights into the evolution of chorionic gonadotrophin

    Mol. Cell. Endocrinol.

    (2008)
  • G. Vaughton et al.

    Evidence for selective fruit abortion in Banksia spinulosa (Proteaceae)

    Biol. J. Linn. Soc.

    (1993)
  • D.H. Funk et al.

    Courtship role reversal and deceptive signals in the long-tailed dance fly, Rhamphomyia longicauda

    Anim. Behav.

    (2000)
  • L.M. Gosling et al.

    Scent-marking by male mammals: cheat-proof signals to competitors and mates

    Adv. Study Behav.

    (2001)
  • L.L. Sharpe

    Handstand scent marking: height matters to dwarf mongooses

    Anim. Behav.

    (2015)
  • H. Goldstein

    Education for all: the globalization of learning targets

    Comp. Educ.

    (2004)
  • L.P. Macfadyen

    Embracing big data in complex educational systems: the learning analytics imperative and the policy challenge

    Res. Pract. Assess.

    (2014)
  • C.A.E. Goodhart

    Monetary Theory and Practice: The UK-Experience

    (1983)
  • M. Strathern

    ‘Improving ratings’: audit in the British University system

    Eur. Rev.

    (1997)
  • S. Haustein

    Tweets as impact indicators: examining the implications of automated “bot” accounts on Twitter

    J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol.

    (2016)
  • J. Maynard-Smith et al.

    Animal Signals

    (2003)
  • J.R. Krebs et al.

    Animal signals: mindreading and manipulation

  • G.E. Hill

    Trait elaboration via adaptive mate choice: sexual conflict in the evolution of signals of male quality

    Ethol. Ecol. Evol.

    (1994)
  • M.S. Dawkins et al.

    Sensory bias and the adaptiveness of female choice

    Am. Nat.

    (1996)
  • R.O. Prum

    Aesthetic evolution by mate choice: Darwin’s really dangerous idea

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.

    (2012)
  • R. Lande

    Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (1981)
  • M. Kirkpatrick

    Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice

    Evolution (N. Y.)

    (2006)
  • M. Kirkpatrick et al.

    The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek

    Nature

    (1991)
  • R.A. Fisher

    The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection: A Complete Variorum Edition

    (1999)
  • G.E. Hill

    Sexiness, individual condition, and species identity: the information signaled by ornaments and assessed by choosing females

    Evol. Biol.

    (2015)
  • A.D. Walker-Bolton et al.

    “Stink flirting” in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta): male olfactory displays to females as honest, costly signals

    Am. J. Primatol.

    (2017)
  • R.J. Weaver

    Carotenoid metabolism strengthens the link between feather coloration and individual quality

    Nat. Commun.

    (2018)
  • R.J. Weaver

    What maintains signal honesty in animal colour displays used in mate choice?

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.

    (2017)
  • R.A. Fisher

    The evolution of sexual preference

    Eugen. Rev.

    (1915)
  • G.C. Williams

    Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principle

    Am. Nat.

    (1966)
  • R. Dawkins et al.

    Animal signals: information or manipulation

  • W. Wickler

    Mimicry and the evolution of animal communication

    Nature

    (1965)
  • W. Wickler

    Mimicry in Plants and Animals

    (1968)
  • G.A. Parker

    Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci.

    (2006)
  • T. Burk

    Acoustic signals, arms races and the costs of honest signalling

    Florida Entomol.

    (1988)
  • P.G. Ghislandi

    Silk wrapping of nuptial gifts aids cheating behaviour in male spiders

    Behav. Ecol.

    (2017)
  • T. Burk

    Signaling and sex in acalyptrate flies

    Florida Entomol.

    (1981)
  • P.R.Y. Backwell

    Dishonest signalling in a fiddler crab

    Proc. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (14)

    • Maternal selection of human embryos in early gestation: Insights from recurrent miscarriage

      2022, Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Next, it is incumbent on the embryo to secrete sufficient levels of hCG to rescue ovarian progesterone biosynthesis until the placenta takes over progesterone production around eight weeks of pregnancy. Human embryos are exquisitely adapted to meet this herculean task; the gene encoding the biologically active β-subunit hCG (CGB) is duplicated six times in the genome and the glycosylated protein evolved to have a much longer half-life compared to the ancestral luteinizing hormone [17,19]. Recurrent miscarriage is associated with a pro-senescent decidual response, characterised foremost by a lack of anti-inflammatory decidual cells (Fig. 2B).

    • Male voice pitch mediates the relationship between objective and perceived formidability

      2021, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      If cheating spreads through the population completely, then the signal becomes useless, and receivers will no longer attend to the signal (Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988). Signalers may evolve to send signals at lower costs, and receivers in turn evolve to better discriminate between a high- and low-quality signal (for review see McCoy & Haig, 2020). This scenario is likely given that human vocal folds are relatively unconstrained by body size, and pitch can be modulated considerably within the constraints of a given vocal anatomy; hence, a lower voice pitch can perhaps be produced with little physiological cost.

    • Origins of music in credible signaling

      2021, Behavioral and Brain Sciences
    • Reply

      2023, Ultrasound in Obstetrics and Gynecology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text