Elsevier

Cortex

Volume 126, May 2020, Pages 26-38
Cortex

Research Report
Testing the exteroceptive function of nociception: The role of visual experience in shaping the spatial representations of nociceptive inputs

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.024Get rights and content

Abstract

Adequately localizing pain is crucial to protect the body against physical damage and react to the stimulus in external space having caused such damage. Accordingly, it is hypothesized that nociceptive inputs are remapped from a somatotopic reference frame, representing the skin surface, towards a spatiotopic frame, representing the body parts in external space. This ability is thought to be developed and shaped by early visual experience. To test this hypothesis, normally sighted and early blind participants performed temporal order judgment tasks during which they judged which of two nociceptive stimuli applied on each hand's dorsum was perceived as first delivered. Crucially, tasks were performed with the hands either in an uncrossed posture or crossed over body midline. While early blinds were not affected by the posture, performances of the normally sighted participants decreased in the crossed condition relative to the uncrossed condition. This indicates that nociceptive stimuli were automatically remapped into a spatiotopic representation that interfered with somatotopy in normally sighted individuals, whereas early blinds seemed to mostly rely on a somatotopic representation to localize nociceptive inputs. Accordingly, the plasticity of the nociceptive system would not purely depend on bodily experiences but also on crossmodal interactions between nociception and vision during early sensory experience.

Introduction

Pain is as an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage (IASP, 1994). It usually results from the activation of nociceptors, sensory receptors characterized by high activation thresholds, i.e., by the capacity to respond – at least under normal conditions – to stimuli of high intensity and potentially noxious (Belmonte & Viana, 2008). Pain has therefore an interoceptive function of warning the brain about the occurrence of sensory events having the potential to damage the body (Craig, 2003).

Among other functions of pain, its localization on the skin or in the viscera is of primary importance because it helps to identify which part of the body is being damaged. Conversely to the classical view (e.g., Kandel, Schwartz, Jessell, Siegelbaum, & Hudspeth, 2013), nociceptive inputs can provide detailed and accurate spatial information, suggesting finely-tuned mapping systems for pain (Moore & Schady, 1995). Surprisingly, studies having investigated the mapping organization of nociceptive inputs in the brain (Andersson et al., 1997, Baumgartner et al., 2010, Bingel et al., 2004, Henderson et al., 2007, Mancini et al., 2012) only focused on the somatotopic organization characterized by anatomical representations of the body surface based on the ordered projection of the receptor fields to spatially segregated subgroups of neurons (Penfield & Rasmussen, 1950). However, it has been repeatedly shown that innocuous tactile inputs can be recoded according to spatiotopic representations, i.e., representations that use external space as reference frame, taking the relative position of the limb on which a given stimulus is applied into account (Azanon and Soto-Faraco, 2008, Graziano et al., 1997, Heed and Azañon, 2014, Iwamura et al., 1993, Shore et al., 2002, Smania and Aglioti, 1995, Yamamoto and Kitazawa, 2001). While somatotopic maps allow coding the position of contacts on the skin surface, spatiotopic maps provide an appropriate readout for the brain allowing to identify the object in external space that is in contact with the body, and therefore planning an adequate spatially guided action towards that object (Brozzoli, Ehrsson, & Farne, 2014). Such complex ability to represent somatic information appears even more crucial for nociceptive and painful stimuli since it allows to detect and appropriately react against noxious stimuli that threathen the physical integrity of the body (Legrain & Torta, 2015, pp. 2–20). Demonstrating the brain's ability to map nociceptive inputs according to spatiotopic representations would provide evidence for the exteroceptive function of nociception (Haggard, Iannetti, & Longo, 2013), whose role would be to optimize the monitoring of space around the body and react to potential danger (Legrain, 2017).

Spatiotopic mapping of touch has been repeatedly demonstrated using temporal order judgment (TOJ) tasks during which participants judge the order of occurrence of two successive tactile stimuli, one applied to each hand, and separated by different temporal delays (Heed & Azañon, 2014). It is noteworthy that TOJ tasks are performed with the hands either in a normal uncrossed posture or crossed over the midsagittal plane of the body. Participants’ judgements are typically less accurate when their hands are crossed, and such an effect is accounted by the fact that the somatotopic representation (“Which hand is stimulated?”) mismatches the spatiotopic representation (“Where is the stimulated hand?”) (Shore et al., 2002, Yamamoto and Kitazawa, 2001). This indicates that, when judging the position of a tactile stimulation on the body, its position is automatically recoded according to spatiotopic frames of references (Azanon and Soto-Faraco, 2008, Heed and Azañon, 2014). Importantly, it has been suggested that spatiotopic representations of touch are not innate but instead develop during infancy (Azanon et al., 2017, Pagel et al., 2009). Accordingly, hand posture does not affect the performance of people with early and complete visual deprivation, suggesting that the ability to remap touch according to external frame of reference is, at least partially, shaped by early visual experiences (Crollen, Albouy et al., 2017, Röder et al., 2004).

The first aim of the present experiments was to test the hypothesis according to which nociceptive inputs are automatically coded according to spatiotopic reference frames. Normally sighted participants performed temporal order judgment tasks on thermal stimuli specifically and selectively activating skin nociceptors. Stimuli were applied on each hand dorsum and tasks were performed with the hands either uncrossed or crossed. We expected a decrease of performance in the crossed posture as compared to the uncrossed posture. The second aim was to test the hypothesis of the role of early visual experience in the development of the spatiotopic representation of nociception. We therefore compared the ability of early blind participants and matched blindfolded sighted controls in discriminating the temporal order of nociceptive stimuli. Considering that touch and nociception share the same spatial representation (Legrain & Torta, 2015, pp. 2–20), we would expect early blind participants to be unaffected by hand posture, indicating that their judgements mostly rely on somatotopic representations of nociception. However, due to the higher relevance of nociception in terms of survival and the underlying fundamental role of its spatial representation, we could also expect that early blind participants would be affected by the conflict between somato- and spatiotopic representations during crossed hand posture. In this line, the spatial mapping of nociceptive stimuli would be less dependent on external factors, such as early visual experience, than that of tactile inputs.

Section snippets

Participants

Thirteen healthy volunteers took part in Experiment 1. Sample size has been determined based on previous studies on similar topics (De Paepe et al., 2015, Filbrich et al., 2017a, Filbrich et al., 2018, Sambo et al., 2013). Inclusion/exclusion and data exclusion criteria were established prior to data analysis, and we report all manipulations and all measures. One participant was excluded because he could not achieve task requirements properly (see Procedure). The mean age of the 12 remaining

Threshold and intensity values

Aδ-fiber activation threshold values were on average 48.08 ± 2.02 °C for the left hand and 47.58 ± 2.11 °C for the right hand. These values are compatible with responses from type II AMH fibers (Churyukanov et al., 2012, Treede et al., 1995). The difference between the hands was not significant [t (11) = .88, p = .400, d = .25]. Similarly, there was no significant difference between the left (M = 53.63 ± 1.58) and the right (M = 53.33 ± 1.76) hands for the stimulation intensity values [t

Discussion

The goals of the present study were to characterize the spatial representations of nociceptive stimuli and the role of early visual experience in shaping these representations. To this aim, two experiments were conducted by means of TOJ tasks during which participants discriminated the temporal order of two nociceptive stimuli, one applied on each hand placed in either an uncrossed or crossed posture. While early blind participants’ performance was not affected by the posture, the performance

Funding

CV, ADV and VL are supported by the Funds for Scientific Research of the French-speaking Community of Belgium (F.R.S.-FNRS).

Open practices

The study in this article earned an Open Data badge for transparent practices. Materials and data for the study are available at https://osf.io/n9kbx/.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Camille Vanderclausen: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - original draft, Visualization, Funding acquisition. Marion Bourgois: Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Anne De Volder: Resources, Writing - review & editing, Supervision. Valéry Legrain: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing - review & editing, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition.

Declaration of Competing Interest

None.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank André Mouraux, Andrea Alamia and Léon Plaghki (Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain) for their help in coding and writing the Matlab scripts related to the experiments.

References (70)

  • P. Haggard et al.

    Spatial sensory organization and body representation in pain perception

    Current Biology

    (2013)
  • L.A. Henderson et al.

    Somatotopic organization of the processing of muscle and cutaneous pain in the left and right insula cortex: A single-trial fMRI study

    Pain

    (2007)
  • S. Holten-Rossing et al.

    Uncertainty about the intensity of impending pain increases ensuing pain responses in congenital blindness

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2018)
  • L.L. Kontsevich et al.

    Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold

    Vision Research

    (1999)
  • V. Legrain et al.

    The pain matrix reloaded: A salience detection system for the body

    Progress in Neurobiology

    (2011)
  • C.E.G. Moore et al.

    Cutaneous localisation of laser induced pain in humans

    Neuroscience Letters

    (1995)
  • A. Mouraux et al.

    Low intensity intra-epidermal electrical stimulation can activate Adelta-nociceptors selectively

    Pain

    (2010)
  • H. Nakata et al.

    Evoked magnetic fields following noxious laser stimulation of the thigh in humans

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • M.E. Nicholls et al.

    The Flinders handedness survey (FLANDERS): A brief measure of skilled hand preference

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • B. Röder et al.

    Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind

    Current Biology

    (2004)
  • C.F. Sambo et al.

    The temporal order judgement of tactile and nociceptive stimuli is impaired by crossing the hands over the body midline

    Pain

    (2013)
  • D.I. Shore et al.

    Confusing the mind by crossing the hands

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2002)
  • H. Slimani et al.

    Enhanced heat discrimination in congenital blindness

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2015)
  • C. Spence et al.

    Prior-entry: A review

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2010)
  • D.M. Torta et al.

    Crossing the line of pain: FMRI correlates of crossed-hands analgesia

    The Journal of Pain

    (2013)
  • C. Vanderclausen et al.

    Investigating peri-limb interaction between nociception and vision using spatial depth

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2017)
  • J.L.R. Andersson et al.

    Somatotopic organization along the central sulcus, for pain localization in humans, as revealed by positron emission tomography

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1997)
  • A.V. Apkarian et al.

    Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease

    European Journal of Pain

    (2005)
  • M. Avillac et al.

    Reference frames for representing visual and tactile locations in parietal cortex

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • E. Azanon et al.

    The sensitive period for tactile remapping does not include early infancy

    Child Development

    (2017)
  • U. Baumgartner et al.

    Multiple somatotopic representations of heat and mechanical pain in the operculo-insular cortex: A high-resolution fMRI study

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (2010)
  • C. Belmonte et al.

    Molecular and cellular limits to somatosensory specificity

    Molecular Pain

    (2008)
  • C. Brozzoli et al.

    Multisensory representation of the space near the hand: From perception to action and interindividual interactions

    Neuroscientist

    (2014)
  • C. Brozzoli et al.

    fMRI adaptation reveals a cortical mechanism for the coding of space near the hand

    The Journal of Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • M. Churyukanov et al.

    Thermal detection thresholds of Adelta- and C-fibre afferents activated by brief CO2 laser pulses applied onto the human hairy skin

    PLoS One

    (2012)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text