Skip to main content
Research Article

Hypothetical Versus Real Monetary Reward Decrease the Behavioral and Affective Effects in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000447

Abstract. A critical question is whether the same decision-making processes underlie task performance with hypothetical and real money as rewards. Across two studies, we administered the Balloon Analogue Risk Task to healthy young adults under these two reward conditions. We found that participants displayed greater risk aversion during trials immediately after the balloon exploded in the previous trial in case the reward was real money, than if the reward was hypothetical money and exhibited greater subjective ratings of regret following losing trials. Moreover, subjective regret ratings after the balloon exploded in the previous trial with real money correlated with risk-taking behavior changes in the current trial, whereas we did not observe this correlation with hypothetical monetary rewards. In addition, when we manipulated the real money amounts to be large or small, participants were more risk averse in the large real money condition compared to the real money amount, whereas we did not observe these differences with varying amounts of hypothetical money.

References

  • Ackert, L. F., Charupat, N., Church, B. K., & Deaves, R. (2006). An experimental examination of the house money effect in a multi-period setting. Experimental Economics, 9, 5–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-006-1467-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bault, N., Joffily, M., Rustichini, A., & Coricelli, G. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex and striatum mediate the influence of social comparison on the decision process. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 16044–16049. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100892108 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bickel, W. K., Pitcock, J. A., Yi, R., & Angtuaco, E. J. (2009). Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: Fictive and real money gains and losses. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8839–8846. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5319-08.2009 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Blais, A.-R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1, 33–47. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bornovalova, M. A., Cashman-Rolls, A., O’donnell, J. M., Ettinger, K., Richards, J. B., & Lejuez, C. (2009). Risk taking differences on a behavioral task as a function of potential reward/loss magnitude and individual differences in impulsivity and sensation seeking. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93, 258–262. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.10.023 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bowman, C. H., & Turnbull, O. H. (2003). Real versus facsimile reinforcers on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 53, 207–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00111-8 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bray, S., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human medial orbitofrontal cortex is recruited during experience of imagined as well as real rewards. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 2506–2512. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01030.2009 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Camerer, C. F., & Hogarth, R. M. (1999). The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 7–42. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Connolly, T., & Zeelenberg, M. (2002). Regret in decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 212–216. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00203 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Daniel, R., & Pollmann, S. (2014). A universal role of the ventral striatum in reward-based learning: Evidence from human studies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 114, 90–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.05.002 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Dean, A. C., Sugar, C. A., Hellemann, G., & London, E. D. (2011). Is all risk bad? Young adult cigarette smokers fail to take adaptive risk in a laboratory decision-making test. Psychopharmacology, 215, 801–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2182-y First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Delgado, M. R. (2007). Reward-related responses in the human striatum. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104, 70–88. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1390.002 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Landers, J. (2008). “Feeling more regret than I would have imagined”: Self-report and behavioral evidence. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 449–456. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Figner, B., Mackinlay, R. J., Wilkening, F., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: Age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 709–730. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014983 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Pchological Bulletin, 117, 39–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fujiwara, J., Tobler, P. N., Taira, M., Iijima, T., & Tsutsui, K. I. (2008). Personality-dependent dissociation of absolute and relative loss processing in orbitofrontal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1547–1552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06096.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Haddad, A. D., Harrison, F., Norman, T., & Lau, J. Y. (2014). Adolescent and adult risk-taking in virtual social contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1476. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01476 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Harinck, F., Van Dijk, E., Van Beest, I., & Mersmann, P. (2007). When gains loom larger than losses: Reversed loss aversion for small amounts of money. Psychological Science, 18, 1099–1105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02031.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Harrison, G. W., Johnson, E., McInnes, M. M., & Rutström, E. E. (2005). Risk aversion and incentive effects: Comment. American Economic Review, 95, 897–901. https://doi.org/10.1257/0002828054201378 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hinvest, N. S., & Anderson, I. M. (2010). The effects of real versus hypothetical reward on delay and probability discounting. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1072–1084. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903276350 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoenig, J. M., & Heisey, D. M. (2001). The abuse of power: The pervasive fallacy of power calculations for data analysis. The American Statistician, 55, 19–24. https://doi.org/10.1198/000313001300339897 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hogarth, R. M. (1981). Beyond discrete biases: Functional and dysfunctional aspects of judgmental heuristics. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 197–217. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.197 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2005). Risk aversion and incentive effects: New data without order effects. American Economic Review, 95, 902–912. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282805 4201459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M., & Patrick, R. (1983). The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: When the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 194–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(83)90120-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kang, M. J., Rangel, A., Camus, M., & Camerer, C. F. (2011). Hypothetical and real choice differentially activate common valuation areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 461–468. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1583-10.2011 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lauriola, M., Panno, A., Levin, I. P., & Lejuez, C. W. (2014). Individual differences in risky decision making: A meta-analysis of sensation seeking and impulsivity with the Balloon Analogue Risk Task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 27, 20–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1784 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lauriola, M., Panno, A., & Weller, J. A. (2018). Regret-based decision-making style acts as a dispositional factor in risky choices. Psychological Reports. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294118786687 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898x.8.2.75 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Liu, Z., Li, L., Zheng, L., Hu, Z., Roberts, I. D., Guo, X., & Yang, G. (2016). The neural basis of regret and relief during a sequential risk-taking task. Neuroscience, 327, 136–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.04.018 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Liu, Z., Zheng, L., Li, L., Xu, J., Cheng, X., Guo, X., … Xu, M. (2018). Social comparison modulates the neural responses to regret and subsequent risk-taking behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13, 1059–1070. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy066 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Madden, G. J., Raiff, B. R., Lagorio, C. H., Begotka, A. M., Mueller, A. M., Hehli, D. J., & Wegener, A. A. (2004). Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards: II. Between-and within-subject comparisons. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 12, 251–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.12.4.251 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 455–464. https://doi.org/10.1007/PL00005491 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nicolle, A., Fleming, S. M., Bach, D. R., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). A regret-induced status quo bias. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3320–3327. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5615-10.2011 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Panno, A., Lauriola, M., & Pierro, A. (2015). Regulatory mode and risk-taking: The mediating role of anticipated regret. PLoS One, 10, e0143147. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143147 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Pluta, J., Hoang, A., & Detre, J. A. (2008). Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: An fMRI Study of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART). NeuroImage, 42, 902–910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.046 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Ray, D., & Robson, A. (2012). Status, intertemporal choice, and risk-taking. Econometrica, 80, 1505–1531. https://doi.org/10.3982/ECTA9042 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reddy, L. F., Lee, J., Davis, M. C., Altshuler, L., Glahn, D. C., Miklowitz, D. J., & Green, M. F. (2014). Impulsivity and risk taking in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 456–463. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.218 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rose, A. K., Jones, A., Clarke, N., & Christiansen, P. (2014). Alcohol-induced risk taking on the BART mediates alcohol priming. Psychopharmacology, 231, 2273–2280. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3377-1 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, B., & Hewig, J. (2015). Paying out one or all trials: A behavioral economic evaluation of payment methods in a prototypical risky decision study. The Psychological Record, 65, 245–250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0112-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, B., Keßler, L., Hecht, H., Hewig, J., Holroyd, C. B., & Miltner, W. H. (2019). What you give is what you get: Payment of one randomly selected trial induces risk-aversion and decreases brain responses to monetary feedback. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19, 187–196. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00656-1 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, V. L. (1982). Microeconomic systems as an experimental science. The American Economic Review, 72, 923–955. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thaler, R. H., & Johnson, E. J. (1990). Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: The effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Management Science, 36, 643–660. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.36.6.643 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wallsten, T. S., Pleskac, T. J., & Lejuez, C. W. (2005). Modeling behavior in a clinically diagnostic sequential risk-taking task. Psychological Review, 112, 862–880. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.862 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53–85. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163633 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Xu, S., Korczykowski, M., Zhu, S., & Rao, H. (2013). Assessment of risk-taking and impulsive behaviors: A comparison between three tasks. Social Behavior and Personality, 41, 477–486. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2013.41.3.477 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Xu, S., Pan, Y., Qu, Z., Fang, Z., Yang, Z., Yang, F., … Rao, H. (2018). Differential effects of real versus hypothetical monetary reward magnitude on risk-taking behavior and brain activity. Scientific Reports, 8, 3712. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21820-0 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Xu, S., Pan, Y., Wang, Y., Spaeth, A. M., Qu, Z., & Rao, H. (2016). Real and hypothetical monetary rewards modulate risk taking in the brain. Scientific Reports, 6, srep29520. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep29520 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zeelenberg, M., Beattie, J., Van der Pligt, J., & De Vries, N. K. (1996). Consequences of regret aversion: Effects of expected feedback on risky decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 148–158. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar