Elsevier

Food Quality and Preference

Volume 55, January 2017, Pages 26-34
Food Quality and Preference

Perceptual and affective responses to sampled capsaicin differ by reported intake

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.08.003Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Burn from sampled capsaicin and recalled sensations were obtained.

  • As expected, burn and liking differed by self reported chili intake.

  • When controlling for prior experience, differences persisted across groups.

  • Burn and liking also differed by variety seeking and preferred heat level.

  • A suprathreshold dose response function for capsaicin is provided.

Abstract

The present study was conducted to a) generate suprathreshold dose-response functions for multiple qualities evoked by capsaicin across a wide range of concentrations, and b) revisit how intensity ratings and liking may differ as a function of self reported intake. Individuals rated eight samples of capsaicin for perceived burn and bitterness, as well as disliking/liking. Measures of reported preference for chili peppers, chili intake frequency, prior experience and personality measures were also assessed. Here, we confirm prior findings showing that burn in the laboratory differs with reported chili intake, with infrequent consumers reporting more burn. We extend these findings by exploring how capsaicin perception varies by reported liking, and measures of variety seeking. We also address the question of whether differences in burn ratings may potentially be an artifact of differential scale usage across groups due to prior experience, and not chronic desensitization, as is typically assumed. By using generalized scaling methods and recalled sensations, we conclude the differences observed here and elsewhere are not likely due to differences in how participants use rating scales.

Introduction

The chili pepper (Capsicum solanaceae) is widely used as an ingredient in many cuisines around the world (Lembeck, 1986), with consumption frequencies that may exceed once per day. Surprisingly, the etiology of chili pepper preference is still not well understood, despite several decades of study. Different motives and reasons have been proposed to explain the widespread popularity of chili peppers. Some researchers have speculated their wide use may be due to the biological or pharmacological properties of capsaicin (i.e. anti-bacterial properties, or gustatory sweating) (Abdel-Salam, 2016, Lee, 1954). Other factors that have been identified include culture (Abdel-Salam, 2016), personality traits (Byrnes and Hayes, 2013, Byrnes and Hayes, 2015, Byrnes and Hayes, 2016, Rozin and Schiller, 1980) and gender (or masochism) (Byrnes and Hayes, 2015, Bègue et al., 2015, Rozin and Schiller, 1980; also see Abdel-Salam, 2016). While the relative weight of these reasons as drivers of consumption remains unclear, it is well understood that chilies elicit a burning sensation. This burn, in the mouth and elsewhere on the body, is primarily due to capsaicin (PubChem CID: 1548943) and dihydrocapsaicin (PubChem CID: 107982), the two main capsaicinoids found in chili peppers. These compounds are potent agonists of the heat pain receptor TRPV1.

The term chemesthesis was originally coined to describe touch and pain sensations that are initiated by chemical stimuli (Green, 2016). Examples of oral chemesthesis include tingling, buzzing, cooling, and warming. These sensations are clearly distinct from classical taste sensations (i.e. sweet, sour, salty, bitter, and umami) (Green, 1996). In regard to oral sensation, capsaicin is one of, if not the most, systematically investigated chemesthetic stimulus (e.g., (Green, 1991, Green and Hayes, 2003, Green and Hayes, 2004, Lawless et al., 1985, Prescott and Stevenson, 1995)).

Despite decades of research investigating the oral burn evoked by capsaicin, response to capsaicin across a wide range of concentrations has not been evaluated in a large group of untrained participants using modern scaling psychophysical methods. Within the psychophysical literature, varied concentrations of capsaicin have been used in many previous studies; unfortunately, it is not possible to extract a single suprathreshold dose response function from these reports due to different delivery systems (liquid solution, cotton swab, filter paper, etc.), type of exposure (sip and spit, sip and swallow, regional application, etc.) and characteristics of the task given to study participants (different scales, or different descriptors such as ‘overall sensation’, ‘irritation’, ‘pepper heat’, ‘burn’, etc.). To identify appropriate doses for use in subsequent experiments in our laboratory, we desired such a function.

One conventional method for estimating perceived heat from chilies is the Scoville Test, which generates an estimate of perceived intensity in units known as Scoville Heat Units (SHU) (Scoville, 1912). However, due to methodological problems with the classical Scoville Test (see (Gillette et al., 1984, Govindarajan et al., 1977, Todd et al., 1977), efforts have been made to improve the method of estimating the burn produced by chili peppers and capsaicinoids. Because there is a simple ordered relationship between perceived burn and capsaicinoid concentration, instrumental methods using high performance liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry (HPLC-MS) and gas chromatograph-mass spectrometry (GC–MS) to determine the capsaicinoid content in chili peppers and chili pepper containing foods have been developed (e.g. (Gillette et al., 1984, Othman et al., 2011, Peña-Alvarez et al., 2009, Todd et al., 1977, Welch et al., 2014). These instrumental methods, which have been validated with human sensory data, are often used as a standard method to estimate heat from various foods or ingredients. One example is work by Gillette and colleagues (Gillette et al., 1984), who used a trained panel (n = 10) with fixed references for ‘slight’, ‘moderate’, and ‘approaching strong’ stimuli to estimate a dose response function for N-Vanillylnonanamide, a synthetic capsaicin analog, as well as extracts of ground peppers (chilies); however capsaicin itself was not included in their report. Their report later inspired two standard methods from the American Society for Testing Materials (e.g., ASTM E1083-00 and E1396-90), but again, these methods were based on trained panels using fixed intensity references. Additionally, many prior studies in this area have focused solely on burn, irritation or bite; however, capsaicin is known to elicit bitterness in addition to burning in some individuals (e.g. Green and Hayes, 2003, Nolden et al., 2016). Accordingly, we chose to address this specific gap in the literature by obtaining intensity estimates for multiple qualities across a wide range of capsaicin concentrations, similar to recent work conducted on ethanol (Nolden & Hayes, 2015).

Greater liking or frequency of chili pepper consumption has been associated with reductions in the reported burn of sampled capsaicin (Cowart, 1987, Lawless et al., 1985, Prescott and Stevenson, 1995, Stevenson and Yeomans, 1993). Based on these data, it widely assumed that regular consumption of chili pepper results in chronic capsaicin desensitization, based on observations that desensitization can occur with exposure in the laboratory and can last over days (reviewed in Hayes, 2016). However, Stevenson and Prescott put forth an alternative explanation that remains untested; namely, observed differences between intake groups may be due to prior experience that influences scale usage rather than true desensitization (Stevenson & Prescott, 1994). This hypothesis suggests that individuals who frequently consume chili peppers have a larger frame of reference outside of the laboratory regarding chili burn compared to those who do not eat chili peppers regularly; thus, when given the same stimuli in the laboratory, frequent consumers use the scale differently, and rate the stimuli as less intense. It remains untested whether differences in capsaicin responses (i.e., perceived burn) across chili pepper intake groups are a result of desensitization due to repeated dietary exposure or merely due to prior context that alters use of the rating scale.

The primary aims of the present study were to a) generate a dose-response curve for capsaicin over a wide concentration range using untrained participants without fixed references, and b) reevaluate associations between perceived burn, bitterness and liking of sample capsaicin and chili pepper consumption groups, and investigate whether this relationship is due to diet-induced desensitization or possible context effects. As secondary aims, we also explored the relationship between sampled capsaicin and a trait-based measure of food adventurousness, operationalized via the VARSEEK scale. Here, individuals evaluated eight samples of capsaicin for their bitterness and burning intensity, along with liking/disliking. They also answered questions regarding chili pepper preferences, intake frequency, prior experience, and personality. This study confirms prior work, and extends current knowledge regarding capsaicin perception.

Section snippets

Participants

Adults were recruited from The Pennsylvania State University and surrounding community to participate in two 30 min visits that were scheduled one week apart at the Sensory Evaluation Center at Penn State. Interested individuals completed a brief online questionnaire to see if they met the following study criteria: not pregnant nor breast feeding, non-smoker, no tongue, cheek or lip piercing, no difficulty swallowing or history of choking, no known taste or smell defect, not taking prescription

Variability in liking and intake of spicy foods among participants

Participants were asked a series of questions regarding intake frequency of chili peppers and foods containing chili pepper, in addition to reporting their stated preference of spiciness/hotness level in their food, and how much they like the burn and taste of chili peppers. Intake frequencies were annualized for chili peppers, sweet snacks, salty snacks, fried foods, and ice cream/frozen yogurt; these are summarized in Table 1. There was no evidence of a significant difference between men and

Discussion

Prior literature exploring capsaicin response lacked details on responses across a broad range of concentrations. Methods used in prior studies have also been inconsistent regarding the specific qualities they measures and used different methods for presenting stimuli, making it difficult to extrapolate across studies. Also, many previous reports focused exclusively on ‘overall intensity’ or ‘irritation’; however, capsaicin can also elicit bitterness in some individuals (Green and Hayes, 2003,

Limitations

Here, 8 capsaicin concentrations were presented across 2 days in ascending alternating order to determine suprathreshold psychophysical functions for multiple qualities evoked by capsaicin. A minimum wait of 2.5 min between stimuli was used, and participants were encouraged to wait longer if any sensation(s) remained. While logistically efficient, this approach has some potential limitations that should be acknowledged. This design was intended to limit i) simple carryover, and ii) sensitization,

Conclusions

In summary, this study expands our current knowledge of perceptual and affective responses to capsaicin. As expected, reported intake of chili peppers was associated with perceived burn of sampled capsaicin, consistent with previous findings (e.g. Lawless et al., 1985, Prescott and Stevenson, 1995). Specifically, individuals who reported consuming chili peppers more frequently reported lower burn ratings from sampled stimuli compared to individuals who reported consuming chili pepper less

Conflict of interest

AAN has no conflicts to declare. JEH has received speaking or consulting fees from corporate clients in the food industry. Additionally, the Sensory Evaluation Center at Penn State routinely conducts taste tests for industrial clients to facilitate experiential learning for undergraduate and graduate students. None of these organizations have had any role in study conception, design or interpretation, or the decision to publish these data.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the National Institutes of Health via an institutional Clinical and Translational Sciences TL1 Predoctoral Fellowship from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences [TR000125], and a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (NRSA) F31 Predoctoral Fellowship from the National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders [F31DC01465] to AAN. Additional support was provided by United States Department of Agriculture Hatch Project

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Cordelia Running, Ryan Elias, Shane McDonald, and David Bolliet for thoughtful discussion of this work, and Gabrielle Lenart for helping to collect these data. We also thank the staff of the Sensory Evaluation Center at Penn State for their assistance in executing the study, as well as our participants for their time and participation.

References (35)

Cited by (54)

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text